Western Kentucky University Western Kentucky University
TopSCHOLAR® TopSCHOLAR®
Masters Theses & Specialist Projects Graduate School
8-2023
Associations Between Parenting Styles & Parental Self-E9cacy Associations Between Parenting Styles & Parental Self-E9cacy
Brian Richards
Follow this and additional works at: https://digitalcommons.wku.edu/theses
Part of the Child Psychology Commons, Developmental Psychology Commons, and the Social
Statistics Commons
This Thesis is brought to you for free and open access by TopSCHOLAR®. It has been accepted for inclusion in
Masters Theses & Specialist Projects by an authorized administrator of TopSCHOLAR®. For more information,
please contact [email protected].
ASSOCIATIONS BETWEEN PARENTING STYLES AND PARENTAL SELF-EFFICACY
A Thesis submitted in partial fulfillment
of the requirements for the degree
Master of Art
Department of Psychology
Western Kentucky University
Bowling Green, Kentucky
By
Brian Richards
August, 2023
iii
ABSTRACT
ASSOCIATIONS BETWEEN PARENTING STYLES AND PARENTAL SELF-EFFICACY
The purpose of this study was to examine whether there is an association between
parenting styles and parental self-efficacy using a United States sample. One hundred twenty-
two parents with at least one child between the ages of 5 and 12 years were recruited for the
study. Participants were asked to complete a survey with measures for parenting styles and
parental self-efficacy as well as demographic information. Results indicated that authoritative
parenting style was positively correlated with parental self-efficacy; while authoritarian,
permissive, and uninvolved styles were negatively correlated. There is a need to replicate these
findings to increase confidence that the results are due to a relationship between constructs and
not due to chance or error. If replication of these results can be acquired then the way will be
paved for future research examining the direction of the relationship and potentially inform how
we approach parenting in the clinical setting to increase the likelihood of positive outcomes for
both the parents and their children.
Keywords: parenting style, parent self-efficacy, parenting, authoritarian, authoritative,
permissive, uninvolved
iv
TABLE OF CONTENTS
List of Tables ……………………………………………………………………...……………...v
Introduction ...……………………………………………………………………………………..1
Methods ...…………………………………………………………………………………………9
Results ...…………………………………………………………………………………………12
Discussion...……………………………………………………………………………………...14
References...……………………………………………………………………………………...18
Appendix A: Western Kentucky University Institutional Review Board Approval Letter……...22
Appendix B: Parenting Style Four Factor Questionnaire....……………………………………..24
Appendix C: PS-FFQ McDonald’s Omega Analysis.…………………………………………...27
Appendix D: Parenting Sense of Competence Scale…………………………………………….29
v
LIST OF TABLES
Table 1. Descriptive statistics for the Parenting Style Four Factor Questionnaire (PS-FFQ) scales
and Parenting Sense of Competence Scale (PSOC)......................................................................13
Table 2. Pearson correlations between parental self-efficacy as measured on the Parenting Style
Competence Scale (PSOC) by parenting style …….……………………………………………13
1
Introduction
Parenting styles and parental self-efficacy are two constructs with plentiful research
behind them. For instance, in recent studies, permissive parenting styles have been linked to the
internalization and externalization of adjustment problems in children (Mendez et al., 2020),
while exposure to authoritative parenting styles as a child is linked to higher levels of life
satisfaction in young adults (Lavric & Naterer, 2020). Higher levels of parental self-efficacy are
even associated with higher infant growth rates (Bahorski et al., 2020). However, levels of
parental self-efficacy has decreased between the years of 1999 and 2014 (Glatz & Buchanan,
2021). There is a notable gap in the current literature linking the two together. While examining
the current literature, only a few articles were found discussing the topic of the potential
relationship between parenting styles and parental self-efficacy (Harpaz et al., 2021). With this in
mind, this literature review largely focuses on research published within the last three years.
Additionally, during the literature review process, it should be noted that many of the articles
found that were published during the specified time frame feature non-American samples.
Parenting Styles
This thesis will be focusing on parenting styles based on the theory originally developed
by Baumrind (1971). In her original typology there were three parenting styles (authoritarian,
authoritative, and permissive), with a fourth (neglectful) being introduced later on by Maccoby
and Martin (1983). The authoritarian parenting style is defined by high demandingness and low
responsiveness, meaning that parents utilizing this style can be seen as controlling as they have
strict rules and expectations while generally not being nurturing. The authoritative style is
defined by both high demandingness and high responsiveness, meaning a parent using this style
has high expectations for their children, but are also warm and responsive. The permissive style
2
has low levels of demandingness but high levels of responsiveness. A parent that uses this
parenting style will set few rules and expectations and often indulge their children’s wants as to
not disappoint them. The negligent parenting style has low levels of demandingness and
responsiveness. These parents do not set rules and expectations and appear largely uninvolved in
their children’s lives (Baumrind 1971; Maccoby & Martin, 1983).
There have been several studies within the past three years examining the impact of
parenting styles in the literature. A study by Mendez et al. (2020) examined the role of parenting
styles in the internalizing, externalizing, and adjustment problems. In their literature review, it
was noted that past research has found significant interactive effects between severe discipline
and problems in children. They used a cross-sectional, paper-based survey. The study consisted
of 422 Colombian parents and 422 children. Inclusion criteria for the study involved the parents
being over the age of 18 with a minimum of five years of primary school and children between
the ages of 8 and 12 years. Additionally, the researchers required parental informed consent and
informed assent of the children. A path analysis was completed for this study to examine whether
relationship patterns were consistent with the observed covariance matrix.
The results found that family cohesion, communication, shared leisure, and parental
conflicts have a direct, negative effect on anxious-depressive behavior in children. Additionally,
parental overload had direct, positive effects on anxious-depressive and isolated-depressive
behavior, somatic complaints, rule breaking, and family dissatisfaction. Impulsiveness was found
to have a direct effect on anxious-depressive and isolated-depressive behaviors, somatic
complaints, rule breaking, aggression, and school/social maladjustment. Authoritative parental
styles had a direct influence on anxious-depressive and isolated-depressive behaviors, somatic
3
complaints, rule breaking, and family dissatisfaction. Ambiguous styles, on the other hand,
showed direct influence on family dissatisfaction (Mendez et al., 2020).
Mendez et al. (2020) reported conflicts at home, parental overload, permissive or non-
consistent styles, and dysfunctional reaction to disobedience play a role in the internalizing,
externalizing, and adjustment problems in children. However, they noted that a democratic style,
collaboration between school and parents, parental satisfaction, and family cohesion serve as
protective factors for psychological problems. Mendez et al. (2020) reported most of the children
in their study live in non-traditional homes. They stated that a democratic/authoritative style has
a direct influence on rule breaking. They reported parents with this style have a decrease in
externalizing behaviors and follow up of rules. A permissive style affects internalizing problems.
This style is connected to children with inhibition problems. Non-consistent styles have a direct
influence on family dissatisfaction. The researchers stated that this style may create feelings of
uncertainty and frustration in children leading to family dissatisfaction.
Sun et al. (2020) sought to examine the relationship between parenting styles and the
self-congruence of people with self-evaluation working as a mediator. They recruited 385
undergraduate students from four universities in the Shadong Province of China to participate in
their study. To evaluate the parenting styles of the participants’ parents, Sun, Xu, and Song
(2020) used the Chinese version (Jiang et al., 2010) of the Egna Minnen Betraffande Uppfostrab
short form (Arrindell et al., 1999). Data analysis was conducted using t tests and analyses of
variance. Sun et al. (2020) used a Pearson product-moment correlation analysis to look at the
relationship between the participants’ perception of parenting style and their core self-evaluation
and self-congruence.
4
The results revealed significant differences in the perception of parenting styles and the
participants’ core self-evaluation and self-congruence Sun et al. (2020). The analysis additionally
showed significant differences between parental emotional warmth and parental education levels,
with parents with an educational level of a bachelor’s degree scoring the highest on emotional
warmth. Sun et al. (2020) noted significant differences in perception of parental emotional
warmth and core self-evaluation between participants who had siblings and those that did not.
The results further indicated that core self-evaluation had a significant, negative correlation with
parental rejection and overprotection. Conversely, results revealed a significant, positive
correlation between core self-evaluation and parental emotional warmth (Sun et al., 2020). An
examination of a mediation model revealed that participant core self-evaluation mediated
relationships between perception of parental warmth and self-congruence, which accounted for
50% of the total effect. Additionally, core self-evaluation mediated relationships between
perception of parental overprotection and self-congruence, which accounted for 59% of the total
effect. (Sun et al., 2020).
Lavric and Naterer (2020) conducted a study focusing on parenting style as a predictor of
life satisfaction in young people (ages 14-29 years). The researchers recruited from ten countries
in Southeast Europe. The researchers provided a questionnaire initially constructed in English
and translated into local languages. Lavric and Naterer (2020) used multiple linear regression
analysis to examine their data. The researchers examined religiosity as a potential confound
variable.
The results of the study indicated that for the children exposure to authoritative parenting
during elementary school was highly correlated with life satisfaction in all countries examined.
Lavric and Naterer (2020) noted that there was no significant correlation between authoritarian
5
parenting styles and high life satisfaction in any of the countries examined. This was also
indicated with permissive parenting styles, with Romania as the only exception. The results
revealed religiosity as a robust predictor of higher life satisfaction. Lavric and Naterer (2020)
reported that their findings indicate that authoritative parenting has a beneficial effect on life
satisfaction among young people, while pure authoritarian parenting has a negative effect. The
researchers additionally noted that permissive parenting styles also tended to indicate lower life
satisfaction unless combined with an authoritative one. They noted that only the positive effect
of authoritative parenting was universal across all countries examined. The researchers stated
that the study appears to indicate that life satisfaction is affected by the presence, or lack thereof,
of authoritative parenting. A limitation within the study was that uninvolved parenting styles not
being included in the study.
Yildiz et al. (2020) did a meta-analysis study on the relationship between parenting styles
and child perfectionism. Their analysis examined 45 studies published between 1990 and 2018.
The analysis found a positive relationship between perfectionist strivings in authoritative and
authoritarian parenting styles. A positive relationship was found between perfectionistic
concerns and authoritarian and permissive styles; however, the relationship was found to be
negative with authoritative styles. Finally, a negative relationship was found between order and
authoritarian styles. This illustrates that authoritarian, authoritative, and permissive styles all
have links to perfectionistic behaviors albeit in different facets.
Overall, the current literature indicates that permissive parenting styles have been linked
to the internalization and externalization of adjustment problems in children (Mendez et al.,
2020), while exposure to authoritative parenting styles as a child is linked to higher levels of life
satisfaction in young adults (Lavric & Naterer, 2020). Additionally child perception of their
6
parent’s parenting style can be affected by their own self-evaluation and self-congruence (Sun et
al., 2020). Finally, child perfectionism is connected to the parenting styles they are exposed to
(Yildiz et al., 2020).
Self-Efficacy
Self-efficacy in regard to parenting can be defined as a parent’s belief in their capacity to
execute parental behaviors effectively (Schuengel & Oosterman, 2019). Schuengel and
Oosterman (2019) wrote a chapter in the Handbook of Parenting, Volume 3: Being a Parent
(Bornstein, 2019) on parenting self-efficacy. In the chapter, they summarized Bandura’s theories
on self-efficacy along with examining clusters of studies regarding the subject. The authors
divided the 788 studies they examined into 5 categories based on the overall theme, or purpose
of the study. The first category examined the effects that parents may have on their children’s
functioning and vice-versa. The second category contained terms associated with high-frequency
in intervention research. The authors noted many of the studies within this cluster focused on
parenting self-efficacy as an outcome measure. Third, the role of parenting self-efficacy across
the transition to parenthood. The fourth category involved operationalization and psychometric
qualities. The final category focused on child physical health. This cluster was reported to have
notable overlap between the intervention and parental transition clusters. Despite examining a
large number of studies, Schuengel and Oosterman (2019) made no reports indicating a notable
quantity of research on relationships between parental self-efficacy and other parenting concepts,
such as parenting styles, within the chapter. This illustrates a gap in the current literature.
Glatz and Buchanan (2021) examined trends in parental self-efficacy between 1999 and
2014. They noted that, over the last 20 years, parents living in Western cultures have a reported
increase in time spent with their children. They additionally reported a decrease in authoritarian
7
control and corporal punishment within these cultures. Glatz and Buchanan (2021) stated that
previous research has indicated parental practices utilizing warmth, positive communication, and
authoritative control have a positive impact on children. They cited that not much is known about
changes in parental perception and beliefs regarding their parenting role as justification for their
study.
The purpose of the study was to specifically examine potential differences in levels of
parental self-efficacy across two time periods (Glatz & Buchanan, 2021). The researchers
hypothesized that their 2014 sample would have significantly higher levels of parental self-
efficacy than the 1999 sample. Both samples were gathered from data used in two different
projects, however, the same measure was used for measuring parental self-efficacy. The data
enabled comparisons between American parents of young adolescents (categorized as children in
either the 6
th
or 7
th
grade) from two different points in time.
Glatz and Buchanan (2021) found that the 2014 sample reported significantly lower
levels of parental self-efficacy than the 1999 sample. These results were counter to expectations
from prior research. Glatz and Buchanan (2021) speculated that some changes that occurred in
the United States may provide explanations for the discrepancy in data. They listed the rise of the
internet and social media as a large factor that may be contributing to lower levels of parental
self-efficacy. However, the researchers do note that this is speculative and that further research
would need to be conducted.
Bahorski and colleagues (2020) examined if parental self-efficacy in new mothers could
predict the growth of their children. Participants were gathered from another study examining
infant care, feeding habits, and infant risk of obesity. The participants were first-time, low
income, African-American mothers between the ages of 18 and 35 with infants between birth
8
and 3 months of age. Infants born with any medical complications were excluded. Self-efficacy
was measured using the Parenting Sense of Competence Scale (PSOC; Gibaud-Wallston and
Wanderman, 1978; adapted Johnston and Marsh, 1989). Since self-efficacy is linked with
positive outcomes, Bahorski et al. (2020) hypothesize that high levels of parental self-efficacy
would predict healthy growth rates, while lower levels would predict slow or excessive growth
rates. The results revealed that mothers of infants that had excessive growth rates had
significantly higher levels of parental self-efficacy than mothers of infants with slow growth
rates. This suggested that high levels of parental self-efficacy may place children at a higher risk
of obesity, however the authors noted that more research would be required to establish a more
generalizable and causal relationship between the two (Bahorski et al., 2020).
The current literature does provide us with the current trends of research on parental self-
efficacy (Schuengel and Oosterman, 2019). Parental self-efficacy has been shown to be related to
infant growth rates (Bahorski et al., 2020). Finally, it is noted that levels of parental-self efficacy
has decreased over time (Glatz & Buchanon, 2021). Some research does exist connecting
parenting styles and parental self-efficacy.
Parenting Styles and Parental Self-Efficacy
Harpaz et al. (2021) conducted a study examining if parental self-efficacy is predicted by
subjective well-being, parenting styles, and help-seeking from teachers. The researchers gathered
132 Israeli Jewish parents with at least one child in elementary school for the study. For the
sakes of measuring parental self-efficacy, they used the PSOC (Gibaud-Wallston and
Wanderman, 1978; adapted Johnston and Marsh, 1989) and the short version of the Parenting
Styles and Dimensions Questionnaire (PDSQ: Robinson et al., 2001) for parenting styles. The
results found that authoritative parenting styles were inversely correlated with permissive and
9
authoritative parenting styles. Additionally, they found that permissive and authoritarian styles
were positively correlated with one another. The researchers noted that authoritative style
positively predicted higher levels of parental self-efficacy, while permissive and authoritarian
parenting styles was correlated with lower levels of parental self-efficacy (Harpaz et al., 2021).
Harpaz and colleagues (2021) noted that a significant limitation of their study is their relatively
moderate sample size and lack of cultural diversity. They additionally reported a need for
replication. In summary, their study indicated that parenting styles may have a large impact on
parental self-efficacy. This adds justification for the current study, showing that a relationship
between parenting styles and parental self-efficacy has been examined in the past. However,
there is a notable lack in replication and diversity of samples.
Purpose
The purpose of this study was to examine whether there is an association between
parenting styles and parental self-efficacy in a United States sample. The research question for
this study is: what is the association between parenting styles and parental self-efficacy? In order
to answer this research question, surveys were completed by parents regarding their parenting
styles and parental self-efficacy, and the scores across scales were correlated.
Hypotheses
H
0
: There will be non-significant associations between parenting styles and parental self-
efficacy.
H
1
: There will be significant associations between parenting styles and parental self-efficacy.
Methods
Participants
10
An a priori power analysis was conducted using G*Power 3.1 (Faul et al., 2009), which
indicated that a minimum of 338 participants were needed for the study for a power of 0.8 with
an effect size of 0.3, and an alpha-level of 0.05. Participants were recruited through Prolific to
complete measures via online survey. Inclusionary criteria for the study is all participants must
be the biological parent of at least one child between the ages of 5 and 12 years. Participants
were given an $8 incentive for completing the survey. One hundred fifty-three parents were
recruited for the study instead of the suggested 338 due to limitations in funding and the desire to
acquire data in a timely fashion. Thirty-one participants’ data were removed either due to
incomplete data or not having children within the designated age range. Therefore, the final
sample consisted of 122 participants.
In the final sample, 84 (68.89%) were White, 25 (20.50%) were African American, 4
(3.28%) were Asian, 4 (3.28%) were Native American or Alaskan Native, and 5 (4.10%)
identified as other. Twelve (9.84%) identified as Hispanic or Latino. The sample consisted of 67
(54.91%) males, 54 (44.26%) females, and 1 (0.82%) identifying as other. The sexuality of
participants consisted of 98 (80.33%) identifying as straight, 3 (2.50%) as gay or lesbian, 20
(16.39%) as bisexual, and 1 as other (0.82%). The average age of the parents was 35.79 (SD =
8.93) years. The parents reported on average 1.97 children (SD = 1.27) with their average age
being 8.68 (SD = 5.09) years.
Procedures
Before data collection, all measures and procedures were approved by the institutional
review board for this study at Western Kentucky University (see Appendix A). The survey
contained an informed consent and debriefing form at the beginning and end of the survey,
respectively. After reading the consent form and agreeing continue in the study, the participant
11
would complete the survey consisting of two questionnaires, and a demographic information
form.
Measures
Parenting Style Four Factor Questionnaire
The Parenting Style Four Factor Questionnaire (PS-FFQ; Shyny, 2017) is a measure used
to identify preferred parenting styles with 32 items scored on a 5-point Likert scale (See
Appendix B). The scale measures the four parenting styles; authoritative, authoritarian,
permissive, and uninvolved, as proposed by Baumrind (1991) and Maccoby and Martin (1983).
The scale has a reported Cronbach’s alpha of .92 (Shyny, 2017). The scales for each parenting
style are not mutually exclusive from each other in this measure. Authoritarian, Authoritative,
and Permissive scales on the PS-FFQ (Shyny, 2017) positively correlates with the same scales on
the Parenting Styles and Dimensions Questionnaire (PSDQ; Robinson et al., 2001) with a
reported Pearson’s correlation coefficient at approximately .82 (Shyny, 2017). The Uninvolved
scale does not correlate with any scale on the PSDQ (Robinson et al., 2001) as the measure only
measures three of the four parenting styles. This sample’s internal consistency coefficients for
the Authoritarian and Uninvolved scales were found to be adequate (McDonald’s ω = .73, ω =
.81).
The McDonald’s omega could not be computed for the Authoritative and Permissive
scales due to a negative covariance matrix. To deal with the non-converging omegas, a leave-
one-out approach was used to adjust the scales (Hayes & Coutts, 2020). The item intercorrelation
matrix was investigated for each scale. For the Authoritative scale, item 7 had negative and small
correlations with other items so it was removed and the scale was recalculated without item 7;
the resulting omega coefficient was adequate (ω = .71). For the Permissive scale, items 2 and 6
12
had negative and small correlations with other items so they were removed and the scale was
recalculated without items 2 and 6; the resulting omega was low (ω = .51, Kalkbrenner, 2021).
See Appendix C for McDonald’s omega analysis.
Parenting Sense of Competence Scale
The second measure is the Parenting Sense of Competence Scale (PSOC; Gibaud-
Wallston & Wanderman, 1978; adapted Johnston & Marsh, 1989). The PSOC (Gibaud-Wallston
& Wanderman, 1978; adapted Johnston & Marsh, 1989) is used to measure parental self-efficacy
with 17 items scored on a 6-point Likert scale (See Appendix D). Reported internal consistency
ranged from 0.72 to 0.82 across studies (Gibuad-Wallston & Wanderman, 1978; Johnston &
Marsh, 1989; Ohan et al., 2000; Gilmore & Cuskelly, 2008). Ohan et al. (2000) reported
consistent good agreement in the factor structures in their sample and the Johnston and Mash
(1989) sample. This sample’s internal consistency was adequate (McDonald’s ω = 0.86).
Data Analyses
Descriptive statistics were computed and Pearson’s Correlation (r) and the coefficient of
determination (r
2
) were completed using SPSS version 29.0 to observe the association between
variables. Pearson’s r greater than .10 was considered small, .30 medium, and .50 large (Field,
2016). Normality and kurtosis of all variables were assessed. A Post-hoc power analysis was
conducted to determine the power of the final study sample.
Results
As seen in Table 1, Authoritarian, Authoritative, Permissive, and PSOC scores were
normally distributed, while uninvolved scores were positively skewed. As seen in Table 2, a
significant negative correlation was found between authoritarian parenting style and parental
self-efficacy (r = -.35, p < .001, r
2
= .12). A significant positive correlation was found between
13
authoritative parenting style and parental self-efficacy (r = .29, p = .001, r
2
= .08). A significant
negative correlation was found between permissive parenting style and parental self-efficacy (r =
-.35, p < .001, r
2
= .12). A significant negative correlation was found between uninvolved
parenting style and parental self-efficacy (r = -.49, p < .001, r
2
= .24).
Table 1
Descriptive Statistics for the Parenting Style Four Factor Questionnaire (PS-FFQ) scales and
Parenting Sense of Competence Scale (PSOC)
M(SD) Min Max Skewness SE Kurtosis SE
PS-FFQ
Authoritarian 18.30 (4.75) 9 31 0.40 0.22 -0.11 0.44
Authoritative 30.57 (4.08) 18 40 -0.38 0.22 0.53 0.44
Permissive 22.40 (3.47) 14 31 0.19 0.22 0.01 0.44
Uninvolved 14.36 (4.89) 8 37 1.34 0.22 3.06 0.44
PSOC 72.34 (12.44) 34 102 0.85 0.22 0.12 0.44
Table 2
Pearson Correlations Between Parental Self-Efficacy as measured on the Parenting Style
Competence Scale (PSOC) by Parenting Style
Parenting Style
Authoritarian Authoritative Permissive Uninvolved
PSOC Pearson’s r -.35 .29 -.35 -.49
Note. All p-values .001.
14
A post-hoc observed power analysis was conducted in G*Power 3.1 (Faul et al., 2009),
where α was equal to .05 and N was 122. The reasoning for this analysis is the number of
participants recruited was less than the recommended amount from the a priori power analysis.
These analyses may be useful in informing researchers whether an increased sample size has the
potential to yield different results (O’Keefe, 2007), or whether null results can be replicated
(Onwnegbuzie & Leech, 2004). A test of Pearson’s r between Uninvolved and Parental Self-
Efficacy yielded an observed power (1–β error probability) of .99. A test of Pearson’s r between
Authoritarian and Parental Self-Efficacy yielded an observed power of .98. A test of Pearson’s r
between Permissive and Parental Self-Efficacy yielded an observed power of .98. A test of
Pearson’s r between Authoritative and Parental Self-Efficacy yielded an observed power of .90.
Overall, these findings indicate that an increased sample size might be likely to result in a similar
amount of variance shared by these variables.
Discussion
The purpose of this study was to examine whether there is an association between
parenting styles and parental self-efficacy using a United States sample. Results indicated that
there was a significant relationship between parenting styles and parental self-efficacy.
Authoritarian parenting was negatively associated with parental self-efficacy and shared 12%
variance. Authoritative parenting was positively associated with parental self-efficacy and shared
8% variance. Permissive parenting was negatively associated with parental self-efficacy and
shared 4% variance. Uninvolved parenting style was negatively associated with parental self-
efficacy and shared 24% variance.
This study found a positive, yet small to medium correlation between authoritative
parenting style and parental self-efficacy (r = .29); where as Harpaz et al. (2021) found that they
15
were positively correlated strongly (r = .51). Harpaz et al. (2021) found that permissive
parenting styles was negatively correlated with parental self-efficacy (r = -.51). A similar
correlation was found in this study but has a moderate effect size (r = -.35), compared to the
large effect size in Harpaz et al.’s (2021) study. Authoritarian parenting style was negatively
correlated with parental self-efficacy (r = -.41). Both studies have similar correlations with
moderate effect sizes. Uninvolved parenting has not been examined in previous studies.
The difference in effect sizes for Authoritative and Permissive styles could be due to
participant samples or the measures used. For example, Harpaz et al. (2021) sample included
Israeli Jewish families rather than United States parents; therefore the difference in magnitude of
relations could reflect a possible cultural effect. Harpaz et al. (2021) noted that Israeli parenting
may be impacted by constant threats of terror attacks and war. Additionally, it should be noted
that the data was collected during the height of the COVID-19 pandemic, which may also be a
mediating factor. Further investigation would be needed to add any credence to these
possibilities.
Both studies used the PSOC (Gibaud-Wallston & Wanderman, 1978; adapted Johnston &
Marsh, 1989) for parental self-efficacy, however Harpaz et al. (2021) used the PSDQ (Robinson,
et al., 2001) to measure parenting styles. While the parenting style measures may examine the
same constructs for the most part, they may put different emphasis on different aspects of
authoritative and permissive styles. However, the results of both do indicate that parents had
similar associations between constructs, which makes it reasonable to make comparable
conclusions about the direction of the associations.
Limitations
16
This study has limitations. One notable limitation of this study is the sample size. The a
priori power analysis indicated that a sample size of 338 was needed for a power of 0.8. While
the post-hoc analysis of this study’s sample (N = 122) indicated adequate power for all
correlations, the inclusion of additional participants might have incrementally altered the
correlations. Additionally, having a larger sample size could allow for a more nationally
representative sample, which would make the results more generalizable to the United States
overall.
There were also concerns of scale reliability with the PS-FFQ (Shyny, 2017). Items were
removed to increase the internal consistency of the Authoritative and Permissive scales.
However, it is unclear if the low internal consistency was specifically due to how the items were
constructed or how the sample participants responded to the items. Furthermore, the Permissive
parenting scale’s low internal consistency is of particular note. Due to more than one item being
removed it is uncertain the correlation with parental self-efficacy is due to error or systemic
relationship. Further psychometric investigations, which include internal consistency could help
clarify how items relate to the proposed factors.
A third limitation is the self-report nature of the measures. Parenting style and parental
self-efficacy scores were limited to self-perception rather than more objective assessments, such
as parent-child interaction observations. This led to inaccuracies in reported parenting behaviors
due to individual’s limited ability to self-observe. This can lead to concerns of test validity in our
findings that would not be as present if a more objective measure was used such as one that
utilizes direct parent-child interactions.
Conclusion
17
In conclusion, this study’s results provided support for the hypothesis that parenting
styles are associated with parental self-efficacy. Furthermore, this study addresses a gap in
literature linking these two constructs. While parenting styles and parental self-efficacy have
been researched in the past, few studies have examined how they may or may not be related.
However, there are concerns of internal reliability within the measures for this study. Other
limitations that would need to be addressed in the future is the smaller sample size and the self-
report nature of the measures.
As such, there is a need to replicate these findings to increase confidence that the results
are due to a relationship between constructs and not due to chance or error. The association
indicates that parenting styles and parental self-efficacy may be influencing one another or have
a connecting factor that warrants more investigation. If replication of these results can be
acquired then the way will be paved for future research examining the direction of the
relationship and potentially inform how we approach parenting in the clinical setting to increase
the likelihood of positive outcomes for both the parents and their children.
18
REFERENCES
Arrindell, W. A., Sanavio, E., Aguilar, G., Sica, C., Hatzichristou, C., Eisemann, M., …van der
Ende, J. (1999). The development of a short form of the EMBU: Its appraisal with
students in Greece, Guatemala, Hungary and Italy. Personality and Individual
Differences, 27(4), 613–628.
Bahorski, J. S., Childs, G. D., Loan, L. A., Azuero, A., Rice, M. H., Chandler-Laney, P. C.,
Hodges, E. A., Wasser, H. M., Thompson, A. L., & Bentley, M. E. (2020). Parental self-
efficacy in new mothers predicts infant growth trajectories. Western Journal of Nursing
Research, 42(4), 254–261. https://doi.org/10.1177/0193945919854464
Baumrind, D. (1971). Current patterns of parental authority. Developmental Psychology, 4(1,
Pt.2), 1–103. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0030372
Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Buchner, A., & Lang, A-G. (2009). Statistical power analyses using
G*Power 3.1: Tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behavior Research Methods,
41, 1149–1160. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BRM.41.4.1149
Gilmore, L. A. & Cuskelly, M. (2008) Factor structure of the parenting sense of
competence scale using a normative sample. Child Care, Health & Development, 38(1).
48-55.
Glatz, T., & Buchanan, C. M. (2021). Trends in parental self-efficacy between 1999 and 2014.
Journal of Family Studies. https://doi.org/10.1080/13229400.2021.1906929
Gómez, I., García-Barranco, M. R., De las Heras, R. R., Martínez de Salazar, A. A., Cobos, L.
S., Martin, M. J. G., & Fernández, C. S. (2013). Memoriadel proyecto investigación:
Evaluación de Estilos Educativos Parentales. Patrocinado por la Asociación Española de
Psicología Clínica y Psicopatología (AEPCP).
19
Field, A. (2016). An Adventure in Statistics the Reality Enigma (M. Kavanagh, Ed.). SAGE
Publications Ltd.
Harpaz, G., Grinshtain, Y., & Yaffe, Y. (2021). Parental self-efficacy predicted by parents’
subjective well-being and their parenting styles with possible role of help-seeking
orientation from teachers. The Journal of Psychology: Interdisciplinary and Applied. 155
(6), 571-587 https://doi.org/10.1080/00223980.2021.1926896
Hayes, A. F., & Coutts, J. J. (2020). Use omega rather than Cronbach’s alpha for estimating
reliability. But…. Communication Methods and Measures, 14(1), 1-24.
https://doi.org/10.1080/19312458.2020.1718629
Hernández-Hernández, P. (2004). TAMAI: Test Autoevaluativo Multifactorial de Adaptación
Infantil: Manual. Madrid: TEA Ediciones S.A.
Jiang, J., Lu, Z., & Jiang, B. (2010). Revision of the short form Egna Minnen Betraffande
Uppfostrab for use in China [In Chinese]. Psychological Development and Education,
26(1), 94–99.
Johnston, C., & Mash, E. J. (1989). A measure of parenting satisfaction and efficacy. Journal of
Clinical Child Psychology, 18(2), 167–175. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15374424jccp
1802_8
Kalkbrenner, M. T. (2021). Alpha, omega, and H internal consistency reliability estimates:
Reviewing these options and when to use them. Counseling Outcome Research and
Evaluation, 14(1), 77-88. https://doi.org/10.1080/21501378.2021.1940118
Lavrič, M., & Naterer, A. (2020). The power of authoritative parenting: A cross-national study of
effects of exposure to different parenting styles on life satisfaction. Children and Youth
Services Review, 116. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.childyouth.2020.105274
20
Maccoby, E. E., & Martin, J. A. (1983). Socialization in the context of the family: Parent-child
interaction. In P. H. Mussen (Ed.), Handbook of Child Psychology. 4, 1-101 New York:
Wiley.
Méndez, J. H. M., Sánchez, J. P. E., & Becerra, M. I. G. (2020). Role of parenting styles in
internalizing, externalizing, and adjustment problems in children. Salud Mental, 43(2),
73–84.
Ohan, J. L., Leung, D. W., & Johnston, C. (2000). The Parenting Sense of Competence Scale:
Evidence of a stable factor structure and validity. Canadian Journal of Behavioural
Science/Revue canadienne des Sciences du comportement, 32(4), 251-261.
O’Keefe, D. J. (2007). Brief report: Post hoc power, observed power, a priori power,
retrospective power, prospective power, achieved power: Sorting out appropriate uses of
statistical power analyses. Communication Methods and Measures, 1, 291–299.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/19312450701641375
Onwuegbuzie, A. J., & Leech, N. L. (2004). Post hoc power: A concept whose time has come.
Understanding Statistics, 3, 201–230. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15328031us0304_1
Robinson, C., Mandleco, B., Roper, S. & Hart, C. (2001). The Parenting Styles and Dimensions
Questionnaire (PSDQ). Handbook of Family Measurement Techniques, 3, 319-321.
Rogers, C. R. (1961). A tentative scale for the measurement of process in psychotherapies. In M.
Stein (Ed.), Contemporary psychotherapies (pp. 184-256).
Schuengel, C., & Oosterman, M. (2019) Parenting self-efficacy. In M. Bornstein (Ed.),
Handbook of Parenting Volume 3: Being a Parent (pp. 654-680). Routledge.
Shyny, T. (2017). Construction and Validation of PS-FFQ (Parenting Style Four Factor
Questionnaire). International Journal of Engineering Development and Research, 5(3),
21
426-437.
Sun, C., Xu, J., & Song, Y. (2020). The relationship between parenting styles and self-
congruence of undergraduate students: Core self-evaluation as a mediator. Social
Behavior and Personality: An International Journal, 48(12), 1–10.
https://doi.org./10.2224/sbp.9479
Wang, D. (1994). Development of a Chinese version of the Self-Consistency and Congruence
Scale. Chinese Journal of Clinical Psychology, 2(1), 19–22.
Yıldız, M., Duru, H., & Eldeleklioğlu, J. (2020). Relationship between parenting styles and
multidimensional perfectionism: A meta-analysis study. Kuram ve Uygulamada Eğitim
Bilimleri/Educational Sciences: Theory & Practice, 20(4), 16–35.
22
Appendix A
Western Kentucky University Institutional Review Board Approval Letter
23
24
Appendix B
Parenting Style Four Factor Questionnaire
(Shyny, 2017)
Read the following statements carefully and indicate your response by marking the
appropriate number.
1. All of the time
2. Most of the time
3. Some time
4. Rarely
5. Never
1 I want my child to follow my instructions because I am the authority to
decide what to do or what not to do. 1 2 3 4 5
2 I would like to be a friend, Philosopher and guide to my child. 1 2 3 4 5
3 I am very soft with my child so that I cannot correct him/her at proper
time by punishment. 1 2 3 4 5
4 I do not have any demand or control on my child and I give total
freedom. 1 2 3 4 5
5 I have little patience to tolerate any misbehaviour of my child or to listen
to the excuses in any kind of mistakes. 1 2 3 4 5
6 I used to understand the feelings of my child in any situation and always
try to get the opinion of my child whenever I buy something for him/her. 1 2 3 4 5
7 Whenever the child comes with low marks, I will not give any
punishments rather I feel he/she will become better next time. 1 2 3 4 5
8 As I am very sad and depressed I cannot show much care and deep
emotional tie up with my child. 1 2 3 4 5
9 I strongly believe that my child’s future is in my hand and so there is a
strict time table for my child to follow. 1 2 3 4 5
10 Important decisions of the family are done together and I give full
freedom to my child to share everything with me. 1 2 3 4 5
11 I give valuable reward to my child for obeying me or behaving well. 1 2 3 4 5
12 As I am very busy with my household and office duties, I get less time
25
to involve my child’s studies or to listen his/her needs and wishes. 1 2 3 4 5
13 I have clear expectations regarding my child’s behaviour and I am not
much bothered about the likings of my child regarding his/her future. 1 2 3 4 5
14 As I understand the strength and weakness of my child, I set some
appropriate rules for him/her and give friendly corrections whenever
necessary. 1 2 3 4 5
15 Though I have definite goal and planning about my child’s future I
cannot follow it strictly because of my leniency. 1 2 3 4 5
16 I have enough stress and strain myself and hence I cannot take care of
my child’s welfare. 1 2 3 4 5
17 I usually like to give physical punishment than giving advices to my
child because I am sure he/she will not listen to it. 1 2 3 4 5
18 I will not force my child in any of his/her future career and I also help
him/her to set a realistic goal. 1 2 3 4 5
19 As I was brought up by strictly disciplined parents, I am very liberal
with my child. 1 2 3 4 5
20 I usually give more important to my own likes and wishes but not
bother much about needs or misbehaviours of my child. 1 2 3 4 5
21 I believe that only through punishment a child can be corrected and I
also do not like to give any financial freedom to my child. 1 2 3 4 5
22 Whenever my child fail to follow the time table given to him/her, I
remind the consequences with a touch of love and affection. 1 2 3 4 5
23 I like to be a very affectionate parent towards my child and also I take
the responsibility of my faulty parenting on my child. 1 2 3 4 5
24 As I am busy and get little time to care my child, he/she is quite free
to move own way to take decisions. 1 2 3 4 5
25 The punishment I give to my child depends upon my mood. 1 2 3 4 5
26 My child talks with me out of being punished after he/she has done
something wrong. 1 2 3 4 5
27 I always threaten my child with punishment but do not actually doing
it because of my leniency. 1 2 3 4 5
26
28 As I am bounded with severe life problems, I ignore my child’s
misbehaviour and I have no idea about his/her life outside the home. 1 2 3 4 5
29 Whenever my child shows disobedience, I scold and criticise him/her
with bursting anger. 1 2 3 4 5
30 Even though I am busy I have enough time to visit my child’s school
& to meet teachers to know his/her progress. 1 2 3 4 5
31 Because of excessive love and sympathy I have showing towards my
child, he/she has no self discipline. 1 2 3 4 5
32 I never like to tell my child where I am going or why I am late. 1 2 3 4 5
27
Appendix C
PS-FFQ McDonald’s Omega Analysis
PS-FFQ Inter-Item Correlation Matrix of Items on the Authoritative Scale as seen in SPSS
PS-FFQ Authoritative Scale McDonald’s Omega as Seen in SPSS
PS-FFQ Authoritative Scale McDonald’s Omega After Removal of Item 7 as Seen in SPSS
PS-FFQ Inter-Item Correlation Matrix of Items on the Permissive Scale as Seen in SPSS
28
PS-FFQ Permissive Scale McDonald’s Omega as Seen in SPSS
PS-FFQ Permissive Scale McDonald’s Omega After Removal of Item 2 and 6 as Seen in SPSS
29
Appendix D
Parenting Sense of Competence Scale
(Gibaud-Wallston & Wandersman, 1978)
Please rate the extent to which you agree or disagree with each of the following statements.
Strongly Somewhat Somewhat Strongly
Disagree Disagree Disagree Agree Agree Agree
1 2 3 4 5 6
1. The problems of taking care of a child are easy to solve once
you know how your actions affect your child, an understanding
I have acquired. 1 2 3 4 5 6
2. Even though being a parent could be rewarding, I am
frustrated now while my child is at his/her present age. 1 2 3 4 5 6
3. I go to bed the same way I wake up in the morning, feeling I
have not accomplished a whole lot. 1 2 3 4 5 6
4. I do not know why it is, but sometimes when I’m supposed
to be in control, I feel more like the one being manipulated. 1 2 3 4 5 6
5. My parent was better prepared to be a good mother than I am. 1 2 3 4 5 6
6. I would make a fine model for a new parent to follow in order
to learn what she would need to know in order to be a good parent. 1 2 3 4 5 6
7. Being a parent is manageable, and any problems are easily
solved. 1 2 3 4 5 6
8. A difficult problem in being a parent is not knowing whether
you’re doing a good job or a bad one. 1 2 3 4 5 6
9. Sometimes I feel like I’m not getting anything done. 1 2 3 4 5 6
10. I meet by own personal expectations for expertise in caring
for my child. 1 2 3 4 5 6
11. If anyone can find the answer to what is troubling my child, I
am the one. 1 2 3 4 5 6
12. My talents and interests are in other areas, not being a parent. 1 2 3 4 5 6
13. Considering how long I’ve been a parent, I feel thoroughly
familiar with this role. 1 2 3 4 5 6
30
14. If being a parent of a child were only more interesting, I would
be motivated to do a better job as a parent. 1 2 3 4 5 6
15. I honestly believe I have all the skills necessary to be a good
parent to my child. 1 2 3 4 5 6
16. Being a parent makes me tense and anxious. 1 2 3 4 5 6
17. Being a good parent is a reward in itself. 1 2 3 4 5 6
Copyright Permission
Name: Richards, Brian
brian.richards648@topper.wku.edu
Type of document: ['Thesis']
-
Keywords (3-
missive, uninvolved


-


I hereby warrant that I am the sole copyright owner of the original work.
I also represent that I have obtained permission from third party copyright owners of any material

acknowledged such third-part owned materials clearly. I hereby grant Western Kentucky University the
permission t

estern Kentucky University policies and the
-
party privacy. I also understand that the University retains the right to remove or deny the right to
depo

use of their


['I consent to the above statement.']