See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/227196334
Performing Gender: A Content Analysis of Gender Display in Music Videos
ArticleinSex Roles · February 2011
DOI: 10.1007/s11199-010-9814-2
CITATIONS
84
READS
10,083
1 author:
Cara Wallis
Texas A&M University
24 PUBLICATIONS428 CITATIONS
SEE PROFILE
All content following this page was uploaded by Cara Wallis on 29 June 2014.
The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.
ORIGINAL ARTICLE
Performing Gender: A Content Analysis of Gender Display
in Music Videos
Cara Wallis
Published online: 18 July 2010
#
Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2010
Abstract This study investigated differences in gender
display by male and female performers in music videos.
Goffmans(1976) conceptual framework of gender display
was refined and expanded upon as a basis for analyzing 12
nonverbal displays associated with subord ination, domina-
tion, sexuality, and aggression in music videos by an equal
number of male and female lead performers. 34 music
videos on U.S. cable stations MTV and MTV2 were
divided into 30-second segments, resulting in 253 units
that were coded for gender display. Findings revealed that
significant gender displays primarily reinforced stereotyp-
ical notions of women as sexual objects, and to a lesser
degree, females as subordinate and males as aggressive.
Implications of music videos portrayal of stereotypical
gender displays and their role in the construction and
maintenance of the gender status quo are discussed.
Keywords Gender display
.
Music videos
.
Sex stereotype s
Introduction
Music videos are an important part of a hugely profitable
and ubiquitous music industry, with over 10 U.S.-based
cable stations offering some sort of music video program-
ming and countless sites on the World Wide Web allowing
for music video viewing and downloading (Grebb 2006).
MTVthe network responsible for launching music videos
into the mainstream in 1981has numerous U.S. cable TV
channels as well as networks in over 20 countries around
the world that broadcast a mixture of local and western
(primarily American and British) music in addition to other
entertainment programming (Jones 2005). It is estimated
that U.S. adolescents and young adults watch an average of
between 30 min a nd 3 hr of music videos per day (Roberts
and Christenson 2001; Ward et al. 2005).
Since emerging in the mainstream in the early eighties,
the music video format has generated a fair amount of
scholarly attention, with research focusing especially on the
harmful imagery found in many music videos and the
potential effects of such imagery on youth audien ces (Gan
et al. 1997; Hansen and Hans en 199 0;Kaloff1999;
Seidman 1992; Sherman and Dominick 1986; Smith and
Boyson 2002). Early content analyses of music videos
consistently revealed a proliferation of stereotypical gender
roles, particularly as these pertained to negative images of
women and women as sex objects (Baxter et al. 1985;
Brown and Campbell 1986; Sherman and Dominick 1986;
Vincent et al. 1987). Many studies also focused on violence
in music videos, finding that males, compared to females,
were more likely to be aggressors as well as victims of
violence (Baxter et al. 1985; Kalis and Neuendorf 1989;
Sherman and Dominick 1986; Vincent et al. 1987).
Research in the early nineties reached similar conclusions
(Seidman 1992; Sommers-Flanagan et al. 1993; Tapper et al.
1994).
Although there is evidence that the most popular music
videos do not have as mu ch sex and violence as is
commonly assumed (Gow 1990), and though certain music
videos of particular artists have been praised for promoting
positive messages, such as images of female empowerment,
most have been labeled as crass manipulations of youth for
presenting fantasies of hedonistic pleasure or gratuitous
violence (American Academy of Pediatrics 1996; Emerson
C. Wallis (*)
Department of Communication, Texas A&M University,
4234 TAMU,
College Station, TX 77843-4234, USA
Sex Roles (2011) 64:160172
DOI 10.1007/s11199-010-9814-2
2002;Jhally1990, 1995;Lewis1990). Particularly harsh
criticism has been leveled at MTV for catering to male
adolescents in its portrayal of gender stereotypes, particularly
those that denigrate women (Seidman 1992; Vincent et al.
1987) or portray leading females in a narrow range of roles
(Gow 1996).
Despite what seem to be similar conclusions regarding
the content of music videos, there has been continued
scholarly interest in this topic (Beebe and Middleton 2007;
Emerson 2002; Railton and Watson 2005; Smith and
Boyson 2002; Ward et al. 2005; Zhang et al. 2008). Such
attention can be attributed to at least two very different
phenomena. First, several scholars have found music video
to be a format that still warrants investigation in part
because of the increasingly sexualized nature of a variety of
forms of American popular culture as well as the
persistence of gender and racial stereotypes in music videos
despite changes in society that have raised the status of
women and ethnic minorities (Emerson 2002; Railton and
Watson 2005;Ward2003). Second, music videos are now
increasingly available, and potentially more pervasive, both
in the U.S. and abroad, due to the ubiquity of music videos
on websites such as YouTube and multiple viewing
platforms, including mobile phones, portable digital video/
music players, and computers.
This study is a content analysis that attempts to update
(though not replicate) earlier content analyses of music
videos through analyzing gender display in music videos
aired on MTV and MTV2, two of the most dominant outlets
for broadcasting rock, pop , and hip-hop music videos to
youth audiences (Grebb 2006). This research examines the
extent to which male and female lead performers in music
videos adhere to conventional nonverbal gendered behaviors.
The research design drew on Goffmans(1976)notionof
gender display and subsequent research (Allan and Coltrane
1996;Kang1997) that has expanded upon his work as well
as prior content analyses of music videos. For this analysis, a
group of gender displays involving hand gestures, body
movement, facial expressions, and clothing were selected for
coding in a sample of 34 music videos. Differences in such
displays based on the gender of the lead performer in the
video were expected.
Music video production is viewed by artists and record
labels as part of their marketing strategy, and marketing
research has consistently shown that heavy rotation of an
artists music video is associated with higher record sales
for that artist (Hay 1998; Leeds 2002). Because music
videos are not only a form of entertainment, but also a
means of advertising, ultimately created to sell a product,
their images are intended to be especially powerful and
captivating (Jhally 1990, 1995; Kaplan 1987). Cummins
(2007) notes that the inclusion of sexual content in music
videos is an important marketing tool.
As part of the broad palette of media available to youth,
music videos contribute to the medias role in gender
socialization, a process whereby the media provide accept-
able notions and models of masculinity and femininity,
which can be observed, reflected upon, and imitated
(Aubrey et al.
2003; Gamson et al. 1992; Morgan 1987;
Ward 2003). Though positive gender imagesthose that
show men and women in diverse occupations and as whole
individuals rather than sex objects or perpetrators of
violenceare certainly found in the media, scholars from
a range of theoretical and methodological perspectives have
argued persuasively that the ubiquity of stereotypical and
highly sexualized gender images in the media, including
music videos, can have negative consequences for the
mental, emotional, and sexual health of youth, especially
adolescent girls and young women (American Psychological
Association, Task Force on the Sexualization of Girls 2007;
Bordo 1993;Pardunetal.2005; Ward 2002). Exposure to
sexualized images in music videos in particular has been
associated with greater acceptance of adversarial sexual
beliefs among young men and women (Kaloff 1999)and
more tolerance toward sexual harassment on the part of some
female adolescents (Strouse et al. 1994).
The Importance of Gender Display and Nonverbal
Behavior
Media of all kinds proliferate depictions of gender, and one
way to analyze such mediated representations is to employ
the theor etical construct of gender display. Gender displays,
part of the larger realm of nonverbal behavior, are the
tertiary sexual characteristics (that are) lea rned a nd
socially created (Mayo and Henley 1981, p. 3). Goffman
(1976) specifically defines gender displays as the con-
ventionalized portrayals of the culturally established
correlates of sex (p. 1). Gender displays can be thought
of as codes that distinguish the way men and women
participate in social situations, and they tend to be viewed
as natural by both the performer and the recipient. Such
depictions of masculinity and femininity are socially
acquired, patterned, used, and understood in relationship
to others. As Goffman notes, One might just as wel l say
there is no gender identity. There is only a schedule for the
portrayal of gender. . . .What, if anything, characterizes
persons as sex-class members is their competence and
willingness to sustain an appropr iate schedule of displays
(p. 8). To Goffman, our gendered behavior, as well as our
concepts of masculinity and femininity, are scripts that are
dictated by our environment that we consciously and
unconsciously learn and perform in order to play our
appropriate roles in society.
Gender display, as an aspect of nonverbal behavior,
includes touch, facial expressions, eye contact, gestures,
Sex Roles (2011) 64:160172 161
and posture. Nonverbal behavior is tremendously impor-
tant, yet because nonverbal elements are generally out of
conscious awareness, they usually go unnoticed unless
extremely exaggerated or in violation of behavioral norms.
However, the strength of nonverbal behavior can be seen in
the fact that when the nonverbal contradicts the verbal,
usually the former is believed (Giles and Le Poire 2006).
Nonverbal behavior is also said to encode power
relations (Bordo 1993; Burgoon and Dillman 1995; Mayo
and Henley 1981). Many empirical studies have associated
power with more use of hand gestures, touching others, and
sustained gazing, and have found that men engage in such
behaviors more than women (Burgoon and Le Poire 1999;
Henley 1995; Major et al. 1990). Self-touch, however, has
been correlated with lower status and as such is more often
associated with women (Carney et al. 2001; Harrigan et al.
1991). Just as there is contradictory evidence as to whether
lower-status individuals smile more than those with higher
status (Hecht and LaFrance 1998), some studies have found
that females smile more than males (Deutsch, as cited in
Henley 1995; Hal l et al. 2000; LaFrance and Mayo 1978).
However, others have yielded no significant results (Carney
et al. 2001). In a meta-analysis of 162 studies, La France
et al. (2003) found that women smiled more than men but
that ch a rac t er is ti cs inclu di n g age a nd natio nal it y as w el l as
situational context influenced such behavior.
In general, it has been surmised that a woman who
demonstrates masculine nonverbal behavior may be consid-
ered offensive because she threatens power relations, yet a man
who acts feminine may be seen as merely idiosyncratic (Mayo
and Henley 1981). As summarized by Henley (1995), there is
fairly consistent empirical support for the hypothesis that
mens nonverbal behavior tends to parallel the behavior
associated with dominance and power, whereas womens
tends to parallel the behavior of the subordinate and
powerless (p. 40). Such implications of nonverbal behavior
in the real-world behaviors of males and females also are
relevant when examining gender display in the media.
Gender Display in the Media
Numerous studies have focused on stereotypical portrayals
ofmenandwomeninthemedia,andtelevisionin
particular has been blamed for creating false expectations
of beauty and body shape (Children Now 19962003; Want
et al. 2009), objectifying women (Signorielli 1989; Ward
2003), and allotting more diverse roles to men compared to
women (Bretl and Cantor 1988; Davis 1990 ; Signorielli
2009). However, a smaller body of research has specifically
utilized the co ncept of gender display to analyze the
portrayal of males and females in various forms of media,
especially magazines and television commercials (Allan
and Coltrane 1996; Belknap and Leonard 1991; Kang
1997
). These studies can be traced to Goffmans(1976)
landmark investigation of the power of visual images in
disseminating nonverbal messages about gender. In his
analysis of nearly 400 advertisements for gender-relevant
behavior, or genderisms, Goffman classified men and
womens portrayals in five ways. In the first, relative size, he
noted that men were almost always taller than women,
presumably to represent their superior social rank. In the
second category, feminine touch, he found images of women
who were pictured touching themselves to convey a sense of
the female body as precious and fragile. In the third category,
function ranking, males in the ads had an occupational or
active role while women usually did not. For the fourth
category, Goffman found several examples of what he saw as
women performing the ritualization of subordination,
including lowering the body in deference, a bashful knee
bend, and canting, or lowering the head to show submis-
sion. In the fifth and final category, licensed withdrawal, he
observed women portrayed more often than men as removed
psychologically (and thus in need of the protection or goodwill
of others). This category included such gestures as covering
the mouth to hide losing control of ones emotions (when
afraid, for example), a finger to the mouth to show anxiety or
contemplation, averting ones gaze or head to show submis-
sion, and numerous poses to indicate withdrawal from the
social situation at hand. Goffman concluded that overall
women were presented as precious or fragile, passive,
submissive, and in need of the protection or goodwill of others.
Goffmans method has been criticized, mostly for what
has been perceived as a lack of methodological rigor. For
example, Goffman did not choose a random sample of ads
in order to conduct his content analysis, and his categories
are not mutually exclusive (Smith 1996). To Cioffi (2000),
he is merely stating the obvious (p. 97). Another criticism
is that gender display as a concept does not get at the depth
to which people do gender in everyday interactions (West
and Zimmerman 1991). Still, Goffmans work has been
extremely influential and has been adapted to address
these criticism s. Belknap and Leonard (1991)extended
Goffmans categories to analyze mensandwomens
magazines. They found that the prevalence of gender-
isms, particularly feminine touch and the ritualization of
subordination, seemed to be greater in so-called modern
magazines than traditional ones. Relative size and f unction
ranking were found less frequently. Kang (1997)com-
pared a random sample of ads in womens magazines in
the early nineties as a means of conceptually replicating
Goffmans study. She used Goffmanscategoriesaswell
as two additional ones and, like Belknap and Leonard,
found infrequent instances of relative size and function
ranking. However, there were more stereotypical por-
trayals of female body display and licensed withdrawal
than in Goffmansstudy.
162 Sex Roles (2011) 64:160172
Other research has analyzed gender display in television
commercials. Comparing commercials from the classic
era and from the 1980s, Allan and Coltrane (1996) found
gender stereotypes still at work, often with little change
between the two periods, particularly in terms of the
number of men compared to women and the voice of
narrators. They also noted an increase in nontypical gender
display of nearly 28% for women (e.g. women demonstrating
masculine traits) but a decrease by 9% for men. Allan and
Coltrane concluded, Much of what we do with gender
display in social interaction is boundary work: we create and
reaffirm group boundaries and construct differences between
males and females (p. 201). In other comparisons of
commercials over time, larger degrees of gender bias and
stereotypes have been found in more recent commercials than
in earlier ones, indicating that television commercials may
have actually regressed in terms of gender neutrality (Bartsch
et al. 2000; Ganahl et al. 2003).
Such research suggests that the concept of gender
display is a useful framework for analyzing gender
representations in various types of media, particularly when
the research focus is on nonverbal behavior. Aware of the
critiques of Goffman, Smith (1996) sees potential for
systematizing and extending Goffmans analytic frame-
work. Among his recommendations are that Goffmans
categories should be made mutually exclusive, and that as
analytic categories they need to be operationalized in order
to be more effective. He also discusses the need for new
types of genderisms. These suggestions will be addressed
in the current study, which investigates the nonverbal
correlates of gender in music videos, a topic that has been
understudied in music video research.
Overview of the Study
Though prior research offers persuasive findings regarding
womens subordinate status and mens dominant status in
media depictions in general, this study seeks to remedy
certain limitations in some of the previous analyses of
gender representations in music videos and to offer a
unique perspective throu gh focusing specifically on gender
display. First, in several studies (Seidman 1992; Sherman
and Dominick 1986; Vincent et al. 1987) only concept
videos, in which a performer plays a role in the story of
the video, were analyzed to the exclusion of performance
videos, or videos that portray the artist simply performing
the song. It is true that more is usually happening in
concept videos in the sense that lead performers can take on
an array of different behaviors, costumes, and roles while
acting in the video. However, it seems likely that an
exclusion of performance videos might skew the research
results, and studies that only take into account concept videos
while claiming to measure what is in music videos may not be
presenting the whole picture. This study analyzes concept as
well as performance videos because gender display, which is
an often unconscious correlate of masculinity and femininity,
should be prevalent in both types of videos.
Second, in a number of the prior content analyses that
investigated sexuality or gender in music videos, the whole
video was used as the coding unit so that behaviors and
characteristics of performers were only coded once (e.g.
Baxter et al. 1985; Seidman 1992
; Tapper et al. 1994;
Vincent et al. 1987). However, because most music videos
are three to four minutes in length, it is very likely that
certain behaviors will be repeated during the course of the
video. Only coding a particular behavior or gesture one
time allows for the potential misrepresentation of what is
happening in music videos. This study therefore divided
each video into 30-second units to be coded in order to
capture a more complete picture of gender display.
The third and perhaps most significant reason to look
once again at the content of music videos is that although
prior music video research has focused on sex-role stereo-
typing, none have performed a comprehensive analysis of
gender displays of male and female performers in the
videos in the manner of Goffmans original study of print
ads or in the mode in which Smith (1996) suggests
Goffmans categories can be expanded and operationalized.
Analyzing displays of the body such as hand gestures and
facial expressions as well as displays on the body
including clothing may shed more light on constructs of
femininity and masculinity in music videos.
Finally, given societal changes since much of the previous
music video research was conducted, there is reason to believe
the gendered content of music videos is in need of a fresh
assessment. Some recent summaries and meta-analyses of a
large body of research on gender suggest that there may be
more similarities than differences in the thinking and behavior
of men and women (Barnett and Rivers 2004;Hyde2005).
Certain trends in popular culture and in the music industry
itself in recent years, such as emo (emotional) rock, riot
grrrls, and girl power, purport notions of gender equality
and broadened ideas of acceptable forms of masculinity and
femininity (Banet-Weiser 2004; Leonard 1997; Whitely
2000). On the other hand, some have argued that postfem-
inist notions of gender equality and female empowerment
have meant that women have become even more sexualized
and objectified in the media (Levy 2005;McRobbie2004).
Others believe that as the overall status of women continues
to increase in society, the result will be less stereotyped
gender representations of men and women disseminated in
popular culture (Gauntlett 2002). However, as the research
cited above indicates, this does not seem to be the case with
television commercials or magazine ads, so there is no
reason to automatically assume this is true in music videos.
All of these trends give cause for a reexamination of male
Sex Roles (2011) 64:160172 163
and female representations in music videos through utilizing
gender display as a theoretical framework.
Research Question and Hypotheses
An analysis of gender display in music videos is relevant, both
for theoretical and sociological reasons, as it potentially serves
to further knowledge of the medias role in the social
construction of gender. Though humans become gendered
through multiple complex processes that are both conscious
and unconscious, the media representations that are absorbed
(consciously and unconsciously), reflected upon, and imitated
play a key role (Aubrey et al. 2003; Gamson et al. 1992;
Morgan 1987; Ward 2003). Modeling and imitation can be
vicariously reinforced through seeing othersparticularly
role models in various mediarewarded for what is
considered correct behavior, including gender appropriate
behavior (Bandura 1986; Bussey and Bandura 1999). For
youth, popular music artists are often viewed as role models
(Raviv et al. 1996), and music videos have been shown to
affect adolescents conceptions of acceptable sexual behavior
and gender stereotypes (Hansen and Hansen 1988; Strouse et
al. 1995;Wardetal.2005; Zhang et al. 2008). Their
increasing availability through a variety of platforms
(Internet, mobile phone, iPod) means that their images and
messages are potentially more widespread than ever before
(Caramanica 2005). Although claims of direct effects of the
media on peoples gender attitudes and behavior are highly
contentious, we know that there is at least some influence
(Gan et al. 1997; Hansen and Hansen 2000); hence,
revisiting the topic of the content of music videos is
worthwhile. As Seidman (1992)notes,Music videos not
only appear to reflect society and its norms, but may also
help socialize young people by communicating ideas about
proper behavior as well as influencing males and females
to develop distinct personality characteristics (p. 209).
This research investigates the representations of males
and females and the nonverbal behaviors that are associated
with masculinity and femininity in current music videos.
The research question guiding this study is, what are the
gender displays of male and female lead performers in their
music videos and h ow do they differ from one another? The
answer could reveal much about the often subtle (or not so
subtle) construction of gender in music videos.
Much of the literature on nonverbal communication and
gender display summarized earlier links certain nonverbal
behaviors with power (Burgoon and Dillman 1995; Goffman
1976; Henley 1995). Prior content analyses of television
commercials and music videos have shown men and women
in hierarchical positions, with men displaying more domi-
nance compared to women (Allan and Coltrane 1996
;
Seidman 1992; Sommers-Flanagan et al. 1993). Because
previous research has also found that televisual portrayals of
gender often lag behind more progressive notions of gender
equality in real life (Ganahl et al. 2003), the first two
hypotheses in this study are:
H
1
: Female lead performers will display more subor-
dinate nonverbal behavior than will male lead
performers.
H
2
: Male lead performers will display more dominant
modes of nonverbal behavi or than will female lead
performers.
Because much of the research on music videos and
television commercials has shown high degrees of sexuality
associated with women (Brown and Campbell 1986;
Rouner et al. 2003; Seidman 1992) and in the case of
music videos, high degrees of aggression associated with
men (Kalis and Neuendorf 1989; Vincent 1989), the third
and fourt h hypotheses in this study are:
H
3
: Female lead performers will display more overt
sexuality than will male lead performers in terms of
suggestive gestures, facial expressions, and attire.
H
4
: Male lead performers will engage in more
nonverbal displays of aggression than will female
lead performers.
In general, it is hypothesized that despite changes in
society a nd in popular culture, male and female lead
performers will demonstrate stereotypical gender displays
in their music videos.
Method
Design
This study examined gender display of male and female
lead performers in a sample of concept and performance
music videos aired on MTV and MTV2. These networks
were chosen as popular television outlets for airing rock,
pop, and hip-hop videos to a large audience. More than 86
million households subscribe to MTV alone (MTV Net-
works International 2004). In 2004 MTV had been the
number-one rated 24-hour ad-supported cable network
among 12-to-24-year-olds for 31 consecutive quarters while
MTV2 was also extremely popular among adolescents and
young adults and reached over 45 million households
(MTV Networks International).
A content-analytic scheme was developed based on
gender-stereotypic nonverbal displays in ac cordance with
Goffmans categories and Smiths(1996) suggestion to use
mutually exclusive genderisms. The prior findings on
gender representation in television commercials and music
videos discussed earlier were also considered in developing
the content-analytic scheme. As elaborated in more detail
164 Sex Roles (2011) 64:160172
below, gender displays initially chosen for the analysis
included hand gestures, body movement, and facial
expressions. Clothing, a gender-stereotypic nonverbal dis-
play adorning the body, was also included.
Video Selection
Music videos were selected using a purposive sampling
strategy. The goal was to generate a broad sample of pop,
rock, and rap music videos popular at the time and that
contained sufficient representations of males and females for
meaningful comparisons. An equal number of videos by male
and female lead performers was desired in order to run a
MANOVA as a discriminant analysis, a test that requires equal
cell sizes to be robust to the assumption of multivariate
normality.
ThirtyhoursofMTVand10hrofMTV2were
videotaped for three weeks during late October 2004 and
early November 2004. Videotaping was done on alternating
days during rotating three-hour blocks between 5:00 am
and 11:00 am, considered prime time for music videos
because most MTV daytime programming is increasingly
devoted to talk shows, game shows, and reality shows.
During the sampling period, it became clear that MTV
broadcast an overwhelming number of videos with male
lead performers, so additional recording of MTV2 was
done during the last week of the taping period. This taping
added only three additional musi c videos to the sample.
This limited number and the fact that only 82 unique videos
aired throughout the taping period suggests that the taping
captured the videos in rotation at the time. The taped videos
were also assumed to be representative of a broader time
period because there is no available evidence that indicates
that seasonal changes influence the types of videos shown;
rather, a given videos amount of rotation is strongly linked
to its songs sales volume (and vice versa). It should be
noted that all of the videos analyzed in this study were also
available on MTVs website and thus potentially accessible
to a much larger audience both domestically and interna-
tionally at any time. In addition, some of the videos were
featured on its home page as well as on Total Request Live
(TRL), a popular show at the time of taping.
The 40 hr of taping yielded a sample of 65 unique videos
of male performers and 17 unique videos of female
performers. In line with previous content analyses (e.g.,
Sherman and Dominick 1986; Sommers-Flanagan et al.
1993), repeated videos were eliminated. To obtain an equal
number of videos by males and females, the 65 male videos
were numbered and a sample of 17 videos was selected by
drawing 17 two-digit numbers from a random numbers
table. The final total number of videos coded was 34. For
ease and comprehensiveness of coding, each video was
divided into units of 30 s, with the average video containing
7.5 units for a total of 253 units. A time display was put on
each video to facilitate coding.
Measure
Gender
In this study, sex was assumed to affect gender display. Sex
was operationalized as the physical presence of a male or
female lead performer in each music video. Given the
findings on sexism and gender stereotypes in prior content
analyses of music videos and concerns about the prolifer-
ation of such images, the focus of this study was also on
gender, but gender display was utilized as an unexplored
framework in music video research. Because the emphasis
was gender display, the race of the artist and the style of
music were not considered in the analysis.
Nonverbal Behaviors
After a process of refining variables, as described in more
detail below, 12 gender displays were chosen to be coded.
Six hand gestures, two body movement displays, and three
facial expressions were selected for coding, as was clothing
of the lead performer.
Procedures
Development of Coding Scheme
This study focused on gender displays of the lead performer of
each video. A lead performer was operationalized as the lead
singer of the song in the video; in other words, the person who
both sang the lead vocal and appeared in the foreground of the
video. Other band members, background singers, and sup-
porting dancers and actors were ignored. The rationale for this
design was that the lead performer in a music video is the one
who is crucial to the performance or the central concept being
depicted and as such should receive most of the viewers
attention (Brown and Campbell 1986; Vernallis 2004).
Before coding the videos, previous content analyses of
music videos were scrutinized, in particular Seidman (1992)
and Sommers-Flanagan et al. (1993), for operationalizations
of gender roles, nonverbal expressions of sexuality, and
physical aggression in music videos. Goffmans(1976)
original analysis of gender display and subsequent studies
based on his analytic categories were reviewed as were
studies of nonverbal gender displays and nonverbal commu-
nication as a function of gender in order to make a
preliminary list of gender displays (Belknap and Leonard
1991; Carney et al. 2001; Goffman
1976;Kang1997;
Signorielli et al. 1994). In the preliminary coding scheme,
subordinate nonverbal gender displays included touching
Sex Roles (2011) 64:160172 165
hair, delicate self-touch, smiling, averting oneseyes,anda
childish finger to/in the mouth. Nonverbal gender displays
expressing overt sexuality included sexual self-touch, sugges-
tive dancing, a sultry look, and wearing slightly provocative or
provocative clothing. These subordinate and sexual gender
displays were meant to address Goffmanscategoriesof
feminine touch, ritualization of subordination, and licensed
withdrawal and Kangs concept of female body display. In the
preliminary coding scheme, ge nder displays conveying
dominance included several posture and eye contact displays,
and nonverbal displays signifying aggression included fling-
ing hands/fingers, showing force, aggressive playing of an
instrument, and showing passion while singing. Because
previous studies found Goffmans relative size and function
ranking to be insignificant, these were not included. A sample
of videos was then viewed and the categories refined.
Coders
Two coders (both white females in their early twenties) were
trained for several hours in the use of the coding scheme with
music videos that were not included in the study sample. The
coders watched each video numerous times and coded each
30-second unit of each video for presence (1) or absence (0) of
the nonverbal behaviors. In addition, clothing of each
performer was coded once for each outfit the performer wore
in the video. Clothing was coded as neutral, slightly
provocative (clothing that reveals more of the chest, abdomen,
or the thighs than in everyday work/school attire), or
provocative (revealing most of the body). Coders made their
decisions independent of one another. Intercoder agreement
was determined by using Co hens kappa, which was
calculated using SPSS. During the training process, variables
in the preliminary design dealing with eye contact and posture
were discarded due to a lack of intercoder reliability. The
intercoder reliability coefficient for each retained variable
was: flinging hands/fingers (.8), show of force (.8), touching
hair (.8), delicate self-touch (.7), suggestive dancing (.9),
smiling (.9), passionate singing (1.0), neutral clothing (1.0),
slightly provocative clothing (.8), and provocative clothing
(.8). Although the coders were in perfect agreement as to the
presence or absence of childish finger to/in the mouth, sexual
self-touch, aggressive playing, and a sultry look across all
segments and across all videos, there was no variability in the
presence/absence datafor example, both raters agreed on
one instance of aggressive playingsuch that both of the
variables were constants, and kappa could not be computed.
Results
A set of analyses was constructed to answer the general
research question: what are the gender displays of male and
female lead performers in their music videos and how do
they differ from one another? This set of analyses was also
designed to test the four hypotheses: Hypothesis 1, that
female lead performers will display more subordinate
nonverbal behavior than will male lead performers;
Hypothesis 2, that male lead performers will display
more dominant modes of nonverbal behavior than will
female lead performers; Hypothesis 3, that female lead
performers will display more overt sexuality than will
male lead performers; and Hypothesis 4, that male lead
performers will engage in more nonverbal displays of
aggression than will female lead performers.
Research Question: Gende r Displays and Differences
The research question asks about the gender displays of male
and female lead performers in their music videos and assumes
differences in such displays. Although the videos were coded
for presence or absence of each gender display in each
30-second unit, to be able to apply parametric statistics to the
analysis of the data, the coding units were collapsed to obtain
a measure of the percentage of intervals in which each
behavior was displayed. A discriminant analysis was then
conducted to perform a multivariate-analysis of variance test
of the general hypothesis that male musicians differed from
female musicians on a linear combination of 11 types of
nonverbal behavior (flinging hands/fingers, childish finger to/
in the mouth, show of force, touching hair, delicate self-touch,
sexual self-touch, suggestive dancing, aggressive playing of a
musical instrument, smiling, sultry look, and passionate
singing), such that males will have a significantly higher
value on the combined indicators than females (type of
clothing was not included). A discriminant analysis was
chosen for theoretical reasons in that nonverbal behaviors
seldom operate independently of one another; rather, they
appear in clusters so it is appropriate to analyze them as a
group (Burgoon and Bacue 2003). Methodologically a
discriminant analysis takes into account the relationship that
variables have with one another, and it reduces the
possibility of generating a significant relationship by chance
(Williams and Monge 2001).
The obtained value of Wilks lambda, .355, was
significant at p=.004 (Chi-square=27.458, df=11). The
canonical correlation between the gender grouping variable
and the new canonical variable composed of the 11
weighted predictors was .803. Significant univariate differ-
ences between males and females were also obtained for
percent of touching hair, F (1, 32)=24.718, p<.001;
delicate self-touch, F
(1, 32)=18.313, p<.001; sultry look,
F (1, 32)=14.08, p=.001; suggestive dancing, F (1,32)=
8.741, p=.003; sexual self-touch, F (1, 32)=5.384, p=.014;
aggressive playing, F (1, 32)=4.392, p=.022; and flinging
hands/fingers F (1, 32)=2.992, p=.047.
166 Sex Roles (2011) 64:160172
Table 1 shows the average of the percentage that each
gender display occurred in each video by a male lead
performer and each video by a female lead performer,
where the length of the video was taken into consider-
ation in the calculation. As shown in the table, the
percentage mean for all displays was in the expected
direction even when not significant. The variables with
significant differences of the means of the percent of
occurrence were: flinging hands/fingers, 51.33 for males
and 30.48 for females; aggressive playing, 13.20 for
males and .00 for females; touching hair, 1.24 for males
and 38.35 for females; delicate self-touch, 5.78 for males
and 37.14 for females; sultry look, .00 for males and
34.56 for females; suggestive dancing, .74 for males and
26.80 for females; and sexual self-touch, 2.69 for males
and 15.57 for females.
Table 2 presents the stand ardized discriminant function
coefficients. Higher scores on the discriminant function
corresponded to higher rates of passionate singing, aggres-
sive playing, flinging hands/fingers, and showing force, all
variables associated with male gender display. The main
anomaly was the positive coefficient for sexual self-touch
because this display was assumed to be associated with
female musicians. Lower scores corresponded to higher
rates of childish finger to/in mouth, delicate self-touch, a
sultry look, and touching hair, variables consistently
associated with female gender display.
Table 3 displays the group centroids on the discriminant
function; the female group had a low positive centroid with
respect to the function and the male group had a high
positive centroid with respect to the function.
Table 4 presents the resu lts of the reclassification
analysis, which shows that the discriminant function was
successful in reclassifying 91.2% of the cases. Note that the
discriminant function was more successful in classifying
male performers than females. Gender display variables
relating to female perfo rmers that were significant by
univariate tests were not associated with the discriminant
function in a consistent way, and some of the female
performers who had higher scores on a certain variable may
have been incorrectly classified as male.
Hypothesis 1: Subordinate Nonverbal Behavior of Females
As indicated in the above discussion, the first hypothe-
sis, that female lead performers would display more
subordinate behaviors than males would, was partially
confirmed. As shown in Table 1, female lead performers
displayed more touching of hair and delicate self-touch.
Although smiling and childish finger to/in the mouth were
considered to be subordinate behavio rs, the findings
regarding both were not significant.
Table 1 Averages of percentages of gender displays by sex of
performer.
Gender display Males Females
Larger percentage mean for males
Flinging hands/fingers* 51.33 30.48
Passionate singing 24.36 14.72
Aggressive playing* 13.20 .00
Showing force 5.10 2.96
Larger percentage mean for females
Touching hair* 1.24 38.35
Delicate self-touch* 5.78 37.14
Sultry look* .00 34.56
Suggestive dancing* .74 26.80
Smiling 16.53 24.70
Sexual self-touch* 2.69 15.57
Childish finger to/in mouth 1.31 3.08
Significant differences between means (α <.05) are marked with an
asterisk. The figures in the two columns show the average of the
percentage that each gender display occurred in each video by a male
and female lead performer, taking the length of the video into
consideration
Table 2 Standardized canonical discriminant function coefficients.
Gender display Function 1
Sexual self-touch .657
Passionate singing .400
Aggressive playing .278
Smiling .247
Flinging hands/fingers .224
Showing force .208
Suggestive dancing .166
Childish finger to/in mouth .219
Delicate self-touch .373
Sultry look .712
Touching hair .772
Positive coefficients indicate displays that were associated with males
whereas negative coefficients indicate displays that were associated
with females. The positive coefficient for sexual self-touch is an
anomaly
Table 3 Unstandardized canonical discriminant function evaluated at
group means.
Gender of performer Function 1
Male 1.308
Female 1.308
The male group had a high positive centroid with respect to the
function and the female group had a low positive centroid with respect
to the function
Sex Roles (2011) 64:160172 167
Hypothesis 2: Dominant Nonverbal Behavior of Males
The second hypothesis, that male lead performers would
display more dominan t n onverbal behavior was not
confirmed. This r esulted from the dis carding of the
variables associated with dominance during the training of
the coders. Sufficiently high levels of reliabil ity were not
obtained for eye contact and posture, and consequently
these variables were removed from the analysis.
Hypothesis 3: More Overt Sexuality Displayed by Females
The third hypothesis, that female performers would display
more overt sexuality compared to males, was confirmed. As
shown in Table 1, females engaged in significantly more
sexual self-touch, suggestive dancing, and sultry looks.
Independent one-directional t tests also showed that female
lead performers wore neutral clothing much less often than
males did, t (unequal variances)=4.14, df=16.411, p<.001.
Females wore neutral clothing an average of 45.28% of the
time while males averaged 98.09%. Females frequently wore
slightly provocative clothing, t (unequal variances)=4.12,
df=16.401, p<.001, with a mean of 44.62% compared to a
mean of 1.18% for males. Though not as common, females
also wore provocative clothing more often than males, t
(unequal variances)=1.802, df=16.655, p=.045, with a
mean of 10.09 for females and .735 for males.
Hypothesis 4: More Aggressive Displays by Males
The fourth hypothesis, that male lead performers
would display more aggressive behaviors than females,
was partially confirmed as males em ployed significantly
more aggressive playing of instruments than females and
engaged in significantly more flinging of fingers/hands.
Results for passionate singing and show of force were
not significant.
Discussion
This study drew upon Goffmans(1976) theoretical
construct of gen der display in order to an alyze th e
nonverbal correlates of masculinity and femininity in music
videos. The findings reveal that gender display is indeed a
salient feature of music videos. As exp ected, there were
differences in gender displays based on whether the lead
performer of the video was male or female, and the gender
displays that were significant reinforced stereotypical
notions of masculinity and femininity. To the extent that
male and female artists perform a song in a video, they also
perform gender.
The majority of the significant findings in this study
were for gender displays that are constructed as overtly
feminine. Female performers displays of touching hair and
delicate self-touch were consistent with Goffmans(1976)
study of gender display in print advertisements, as well as
with Belknap and Leonards(1991) and Kangs(1997)
findings regarding gender displays of feminine touch in
magazines. They also align with previous studies of MTV
that found women portrayed in a more submissive manner
(Seidman 1992; Vincent et al. 1987). Such data reveal that
despite womens gains in equali ty in the real world, and
despite hopes for less stereotypical gender representations
in popular culture, in the realm of music videos women are
still depicted as more fragile and thus in need of the
protection of men.
In contrast to some earlier studies (e.g., Sommers-Flanagan
et al. 1993), male performers were not observed engaging in
sexual behavior. It is interesting to note that such findings are
consistent with the portrayal of males in video games
according to a recent content analysis (Pieper et al. 2005).
However, considering that three of the five significant
displays for females (sexual self-touch, suggestive dancing,
and sultry look) were of sexual behavior, the message given
by and about women in these videos seems to be that
sexually suggestive behavior is normal and appropriate for
women but not necessarily for men. Images of female lead
performers dancing suggestively or touching themselves in a
sexual manner drive home the point that women are sexual
objects, ready to be consumed by men. The clothing worn in
the videos also communicates this message. Nearly all the
male performers in the videos wore neutral clothing while
provocative clothing was worn by the majority of women.
This result differs from Vincent et al.s(1987) finding, where
the sex of the performer was not correlated with seductive
dress, and it reveals an increase compared to Seidmans
(1992) content analysis of music videos in which one third of
females wore revealing attire. It also affirms numerous
studies of television in general and television commercials
in particular that have found that women are expected to be
sexy while men do not have to meet such demands. The
same complaint has been raised numerous times in relation
to the music industry (Coates 1997;Leonard2007).
In terms of aggressive behavior, males were significantly
more likely to engage in aggressive playing of an
instrument while no females manifested such behavior.
Table 4 Classification results.
Gender of performer Predicted group membership Total
Male Female
Male 17 (100%) 0 17
Female 3 (17.6%) 14 (82.4%) 17
91.2% of original cases were correctly classified. The discriminant
function was more successful in classifying male performers than
female performers
168 Sex Roles (2011) 64:160172
Males were also more likely than women to fling their
fingers or hands in an aggressive manner. The message
seems to be that just as women are expected to act sexually,
males should engage in more typically masculine aggres-
sive behavior. These findings are consistent with all of the
music video research reviewed earlier.
Taken as a whole, this research has two main implica-
tions. The first is that the theoretical construct o f gender
display is a useful framework for analyzing mediated
representations of masculinity and femi ninity. A focus on
gender display h elps reveal how gender is constructed
nonverbally in the media. To return to Goffman, an analysis
of gender display is a means of understanding how the
appropriate schedule of displays is sustained. Nonverbal
cues are often taken for granted and out of conscious
awareness, yet bringing them to the forefront of investiga-
tion is an important task. The fact that gender displays, like
all nonverbal behavior, are complex and interrelated, with
several cues acting simultaneously rather than in isolation,
means that their overall impression can be quite profound,
particularly when they are presented as the norm and in a
powerful medium such as a music video.
Though Goffmans(1976) categories served as the initial
basis for this analysis, in the process of developing the
coding scheme it became clear that only gender displays
associated with what he termed feminine touch and
ritualization of subordination should be operationalized.
Furthermore, whereas those displays associated with fem-
inine touch (touching hair and delicate self touch) were
found to be significant in the analysis, this was not true of
displays associated with the ritualization of subordination
(smiling, childish finger to/in mouth). This finding suggests
that in our current media environment, an analysis of gender
display must place greater emphasis on those displays that
express female sexuality. The corollary for males is found in
displays that express aggression.
The second implication of this research is that analyzing
gender display reveals the institutionalized sexism that
persists in media representations. Overall, the findi ngs
regarding gender display in music videos communicate,
once again, a distorted message as to females’“proper role
in societyas sexual and subordinateand, to a lesser
degree, males’“proper role as wellas aggressive. Solely
in terms of who is shown, the difficulty in obtaining an
equal number of videos by male and female lead performers
reveals the dominance of males on music television to a
much larger degree than in previous content analyses of
MTV (65 male videos compared to 17 female videos in
40 hr of videotaping).
These findings, unfortunately, are not surprising because
music videos are a form of entertainment as well as a means
of advertising, and numerous studies have found stereo-
typed gender behavior in both. At the same time, as more
female performers have claimed equality with men and
have sought to break down gender stereotypes in the music
industry, the degree of stereotyped gender display in music
videos raises concern as to the impact women have actually
had in shifting this very gendered terrain. Although most
musical artists do not produce their own music videos, in
assuming a highly gendered identity, in performing gender,
these artists are participa ting in the maintenance and
production of the gender status quo. For the most part,
they are not challenging the established gender order but
are reproducing socially constructed gender hierarchies. If it
is true that gender-deviant behavior is punished (Mayo and
Henley 1981), then in such a highly commercialized realm
as the music industry, when deviance could possibly mean
the loss of a career, perhaps it is no wonder highly gendered
behavior is consi stently observed. Certainly there are artists
going against the grain of gender stereotypes, but as this
study reveals, they are in the min ority.
This study suffers from some limitations, one of which
was a relatively small sample size (34 videos). The design
of the study required an equal number of videos by males
and females, but this was difficult to achieve due to the
predominance of videos on MTV and MTV2 by male lead
performers. Also, though the study included a robust and
varied set of videos, it was not a random sample. Another
limitation was that neither the race of the performer nor the
musical genre were considered in the analysis though
clearly both can inform the content of m usic videos
(Emerson 2002; Tapper et al. 1994). Furthermore, though
a content analysis seems to be an objective measurement, it
is limited by the fact that characteristics of observers can
affect what they see. This is true in regard s to both the
categories selected by the researcher for coding as well as
the subjectivity of the coders. The study obtained high
indexes of reliability for the coding variables; however,
certain variables had to be eliminated to achieve such
reliability, resulting in fewer data. It also must be
acknowledged that the raters were relatively homogeneous
in terms of age, sex, education level, and socioeconomic
status. Finally, any study of music videos is influenced by
what is in rotation at a particular moment on a particular
channel and in terms of the performer and the style of the
song. For example, an artist who behaves aggressively in an
up-tempo song may display more passive behavior in a
slow song.
Although only slightly more than half of the gender
displays in this study were found to be significant, all of the
behaviors were in the predicted direction for gender behavior.
Gender display can thus be a useful theoretical tool for
analyzing gender representations in the media. Further
research would benefit by exploring an expanded range of
gender displays in a larger sample of videos and including the
race/ethnicity of the performer and the genre of music in the
Sex Roles (2011) 64:160172 169
analysis. Given the role of mediated representations in the
social construction of gender and the way music videos prime
viewers mental schemas (Hansen and Hansen 2000), these
results could have important implications.
Acknowledgements An earlier version of this paper was presented
at the National Communication Association Annual Meeting in
Boston, November 2005. The author would like to thank three
anonymous reviewers, Irene Frieze, and Rena Rudy for their careful
readings and helpful comments on this paper. Heartfelt gratitude as
well goes to Margaret McLaughlin for her guidance on several drafts
of the manuscript. I also wish to thank Josh Barbour and Xi Cui for
assistance with data analysis.
References
Allan, K., & Coltrane, S. (1996). Gender displaying television
commercials: A comparative study of television commercials in
the 1950s and 1980s. Sex Roles, 35, 185203.
American Academy of Pediatrics. (1996). Impact of music lyrics and
music videos on children and youth (RE9648). Pediatrics, 98 ,
12191221.
American Psychological Association, Task Force on the Sexualization
of Girls. (2007). Repo rt of the APA Task Force on the
Sexualization of Girls. Retrieved from www.apa.org/pi/wpo/
sexualization.html
Aubrey, J. S., Harrison, K., Kramer, L., & Yellin, J. (2003). Variety vs.
timing: Gender differences in college students sexual expect-
ations as predicted by exposure to sexually oriented television.
Communication Research, 30, 432460.
Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: A
social cognitive theory. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice Hall.
Banet-Weiser, S. (2004). Girls rule! Gender, feminism and Nickelodeon.
Critical Studies in Media Communication, 21,119131.
Barnett, R., & Rivers, C. (2004). Same difference: How gender myths
are hurting our relationships, our children, and our jobs. New
York: Basic Books.
Bartsch, R. A., Burnett, T., Diller, T. R., & Rankin-Williams, E. (2000).
Gender representation in television commercials: Updating an
update. Sex Roles, 43,735743.
Baxter, R., De Riemer, C., Landini, A., Leslie, L., & Singletary, M. W.
(1985). A content analysis of music videos. Journal of
Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 29, 333340.
Beebe, R., & Middleton, J. (Eds.). (2007). Medium cool: Music videos
from soundies to cellphones. Durham: Duke University Press.
Belknap, P., & Leonard, W. M., II. (1991). A conceptual replication
and extension of Erving Goffmans study of gender advertise-
ments. Sex Roles, 25, 103118.
Bordo, S. (1993). Unbearable weight: Feminism, culture and the
body. Berkeley: University of California Press.
Bretl, D. J., & Cantor, J. (1988). The portrayal of men and women in
U.S. television commercials: A recent content analysis and trends
over 15 years. Sex Roles, 18, 595609.
Brown, J. D., & Campbell, K. (1986). Race and gender in music
videos: The same beat but a different drummer. Journal of
Communication, 36,94106.
Burgoon, J. K., & Bacue, A. (2003). Nonverbal communication skills.
In J. O. Greene & B. R. Burleson (Eds.), Handbook of
communication and social interaction skills (pp. 179220).
New York: Routledge.
Burgoon, J. K., & Dillman, L. (1995). Gender, immediacy, and
nonverbal communication. In P. J. Kalbfleisch & M. J. Cody
(Eds.), Gender, power, and communication in human relationships
(pp. 6381). Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Burgoon, J. K., & Le Poire, B. A. (1999). Nonverbal cues and
interpersonal judgments: Participant and observer perceptions of
intimacy, dominance, composure, and formality.
Communication
Monographs, 66, 105124.
Bussey, K., & Bandura, A. (1999). Social cognitive theory of gender
development and differentiation. Psychological Review, 106,
676713.
Caramanica, J. (2005). I screen, you screen. The new age of music
video. New York Times. Retrieved from http://www.nytimes.com/
2005/07/31/arts/music/31cara.html?th&emc=th
Carney,D.R.,Hall,J.A.,&SmithLeBeau,L.(2001).Beliefs
about the nonverbal expression of social power. Unpublished
manuscript.
Children Now. (19962003). Reflections of girls in the media: A two-
part study on gender and media. Retrieved from http://www.
childrennow.org/media/mc97/ReflectSummary.html
Cioffi, F. (2000). Stating the obvious: What does Erving Goffman
really tell us? In G. A. Fine & G. W. H. Smith (Eds.), Erving
Goffman (pp. 97104). London: Sage.
Coates, N. (1997). (R)evolution now? Rock and the political potential
of gender. In S. Whitely (Ed.), Sexing the groove: Popular music
and gender (pp. 5064). London: Routledge.
Cummins, R. G. (2007). Selling music with sex: The content and
effects of sex in music videos on viewer enjoyment. Journal of
Promotion Management, 13,95109.
Davis, D. M. (1990). Portrayals of women in prime-time network
television: some demographic characteristics. Sex Roles, 23, 325
332.
Emerson, R. A. (2002). Where my girls at? Negotiating black
womanhood in music videos. Gender & Society, 16,115135.
Gamson, W. A., Croteau, D., Hoynes, W., & Sasson, T. (1992). Media
images and the social construction of reality. Annual Review of
Sociology, 18, 373393.
Gan, S., Zillmann, D., & Mitrook, M. (1997). Stereotyping effect of
black womans sexual rap on white audiences. Basic and Applied
Psychology, 19, 381399.
Ganahl, D. J., Prinsen, T. J., & Netzley, S. B. (2003). A content
analysis of prime time commercials: A contextual framework of
gender representation. Sex Roles, 49, 545551.
Gauntlett, D. (2002). Media, gender and identity. London: Routledge.
Giles, H., & Le Poire, B. A. (2006). Introduction: The ubiquity and
social meaningfulness of nonverb al communicat ion. In V.
Manusov & M. L. Patt erson (Eds.), The Sage hand bo ok of
nonverbal communication (pp. xvxxvii). Thousand Oaks:
Sage.
Goffman, E. (1976). Gender advertisements. New York: Harper
Colophon Books.
Gow, J. (1990). The relationship between violent and sexual images
and the popularity of music videos. Popular Music & Society, 14
(4), 110.
Gow, J. (1996). Reconsidering gender roles on MTV: Depictions in the
most popular music videos of the early 1990s. Communication
Reports, 9,151161.
Grebb, M. (2006). Rock, rap & revenue. Cable Fax Magazine.
Retrieved from http://www.cablefax.com/cablefaxmag/business/
competition/web/18707.html
Hall,J.A.,Carter,J.D.,&Horgan,T.G.(2000).Genderdifferences
in nonverbal communication of emotion. In A. H. Fischer (Ed.),
Gender and emoti on: Social psychological pers pectives (pp.
97117). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Hansen, C. H., & Hansen, R. D. (1988). How rock music videos
can change what is seen when boy meets girl: Pri ming
stereotypic appraisal of social interactions. Sex Roles, 19,
287316.
170 Sex Roles (2011) 64:160172
Hansen, C. H., & Hansen, R. D. (1990). The influence of sex and
violence on the appeal of rock music videos. Communication
Research, 17, 212234.
Hansen, C. H., & Hansen, R. D. (2000). Music and music videos. In
D. Zillmann & P. Vorderer (Eds.), Media entertainment: The
psychology of its appeal (pp. 175196). Mahwah: Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates.
Harrigan, J. A., Lucic, K. S., Kay, D., McLaney, A., & Rosenthal, R.
(1991). Effect of expresser role and type of self-touching on
observers perceptions. Journal of Applied Social Psychology,
21, 585609.
Hay, C. (1998, September 26). Viewer opinions sought. Billboard,5
6.
Hecht, M., & LaFrance, M. (1998). License or obligation to smile:
The effect of power and sex on amount and type of smiling.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24, 13321342.
Henley, N. (1995). Body politics revisited: What do we know today?
In P. J. Kalbfleisch & M. J. Cody (Eds.), Gender, power, and
communication in human relationships (pp. 2761). Hillsdale:
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Hyde, J. S. (2005). The gender similarities hypothesis. American
Psychologist, 60, 581592.
Jhally, S. (Writer/Director/Producer). (1990). Dreamworlds: Gender/
sex/power in rock video [Videotape]. United States.
Jhally, S. (Writer/Director/Producer). (1995). Dreamworlds II: Gender/
sex/power in music video. [Videotape]. United States.
Jones, S. (2005). MTV: The medium was the message. Critical
Studies in Media Communication, 22,8388.
Kalis, P., & Neuendorf, K. A. (1989). Aggressive cue prominence and
gender participation in MTV. Journalism Quarterly, 66, 148
154. 229.
Kaloff, L. (1999). The effects of gender and music video imagery on
sexual attitudes. The Journal of Social Psychology, 139, 378
385.
Kang, M.-E. (1997). The portrayal of womens images in magazine
advertisements: Goffmans gender analysis revisited. Sex Roles,
37, 979996.
Kaplan, E. A. (1987). Rocking around the clock: Music television,
postmodernism, and consumer culture. New York: Methuen.
LaFrance, M., & Mayo, C. (1978). Moving bodies: Nonverbal
communication in social relationships. Monterey: Brooks.
LaFrance, M., Hecht, M. A., & Paluck, E. L. (2003). The contingent
smile: A meta-analysis of sex differences. Psychological Bulletin,
129, 305334.
Leeds, J. (2002, December 18). MTVs playlist to focus more on
popular acts. Los Angeles Times, p. C2.
Leonard, M. (1997). Rebel girl, you are the queen of my world:
Feminism, subculture and grrrl power. In S. Whitely (Ed.),
Sexing the groove: Popular music and gender (pp. 230255).
New York: Routledge.
Leonard, M. (2007). Gender in the music industry: Rock, discourse
and girl power. Burlington: Ashgate.
Levy, A. (2005). Female chauvinist pigs: Women and the rise of
raunch culture. New York: Free.
Lewis, L. (1990). Gender politics and MTV: Voicing the difference.
Philadelphia: Temple University Press.
Major, B., Schmidlin, A. M., & Williams, L. (1990). Gender patterns
in social touch: The impact of setting and age. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 634643.
Mayo, C., & Henley, N. M. (1981). Nonverbal behavior: Barrier or
agent for sex role change? In C. Mayo & N. M. Henley (Eds.),
Gender and nonverbal behavior (pp. 313). New York: Springer-
Verlag.
McRobbie, A. (2004). Notes on postfeminism and popular culture:
Bridget Jones and the new gender regime. In A. Harris (Ed.), All
about the girl: Culture, power, and identity (pp. 313). New
York: Routledge.
Morgan, M. (1987). Television, sex-role attitudes, and sex-role
behavior. Journal of Early Adolescence, 7, 269282.
MTV Networks International. (2004). MTV global factsheet. Retrieved
fro m http://www.mcdepk.com/mtvpartnership/downloads/
mtv_global_fact_sheet.p df
Pardun,C.J.,LEngl e, K. L., & Brown, J. D. (2005). Link ing
exposure to outcomes: Early adolescents consumption of
sexual content in six media. Mass Communicati on & Society,
8,7591.
Pieper, K. M., Chan, E., & Smith, S. L. (2005, June). Methodological
issues in content analyses of video games. Paper presented at the
meeting of the International Communication Association, New
York, New York.
Railton, D., & Watson, P. (2005). Naughty girls and red blooded
women: Representations of female heterosexuality in music
video. Feminist Media Studies, 5,5163.
Raviv, A., Bar-Tal, D., Raviv, A., & Ben-Horin, A. (1996). Adolescent
idolization of pop singers: Causes, expression and reliance.
Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 25, 631650.
Roberts, D. F., & Christenson, P. G. (2001). Popular music in
childhood and adolescence. In D. G. Singer & J. L. Singer (Eds.),
Handbook of children and the media (pp. 395414). Thousand
Oaks: Sage.
Rouner, D., Slater, M. D., & Domenech-Rodriguez, M. (2003).
Adolescent evaluation of gender role and sexual imagery in
television advertisements. Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic
Media, 47, 435454.
Seidman, S. A. (1992). An investigation of sex-role stereotyping in
music videos. Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 36,
209216.
Sherman, B. L., & Dominick, J. R. (1986). Violence and sex in music
videos: TV and rock n roll. Journal of Communication, 36,79
92.
Signorielli, N. (1989). Television and conceptions about sex roles:
Maintaining conventionality and the status quo. Sex Roles, 21,
341360.
Signorielli, N. (2009). Race and sex in prime time: A look at
occupations and occupational prestige. Mass Communication and
Society, 12, 332352.
Signorielli, N., McLeod, D., & Healy, E. (1994). Gender stereotypes
in MTV commercials: the beat goes on. Journal of Broadcasting
& Electronic Media, 38,91101.
Smith, G. (1996). Gender advertisements revisited: A visual sociology
classic. Electronic Journal of Sociology. Retrieved from http://
www.sociology.org/content/vol002.001/smith.html
Smith, S. L., & Boyson, A. R. (2002). Violence in music videos:
Examining the prevalence and context of physical aggression.
Journal of Communication, 52,6183.
Sommers-Flanagan, R., Sommers-Flanagan, J., & Davis, B. (1993).
Whats happening on music television? A gender role content
analysis. Sex Roles, 28, 745753.
Strouse, J. S., Goodwin, M. P., & Roscoe, B. (1994). Correlates of
attitudes toward sexual harassment among early adolescents. Sex
Roles, 31, 559577.
Strouse, J. S., Buerkal-Rothfuss, N. L., & Long, E. C. (1995). Gender
and family as moderators of the relationship between music video
exposure and adolescent sexual permissiveness. Adolescence, 30,
505521.
Tapper, J., Thorson, E., & Black, D. (1994). Variations in music
videos as a function of their musical genre. Journal of
Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 38, 103113.
Vernallis, C. (2004). Experiencing music video: Aesthetics and
cultural context. New York: Columbia University Press.
Sex Roles (2011) 64:160172 171
Vincent, R. C. (1989). Clios consciousness raised? Portrayal of women
in rock videos re-examined. Journalism Quarterly, 66, 155160.
Vincent, R. C., Davis, D. K., & Boruszkowski, L. A. (1987). Sexism
on MTV: The portrayal of women in rock videos. Journalism
Quarterly, 64, 750755. 941.
Want, S. C., Vickers, K., & Amos, J. (2009). The influence of television
programs on appearance satisfaction: Making and mitigating
social comparisons to Friends. Sex Roles, 60, 642654.
Ward, L. M. (2002). Does television exposure affect emerging adults
attitudes and assumptions about sexual relationships? Correlation
and experimental confirmation. Journal of Youth and Adolescence,
31,115.
Ward, L. M. (2003). Understanding the role of entertainment media in
the sexual socialization of A merican youth: A review of
empirical research. Developmental Review, 23, 347388.
Ward, L. M., Hansbrough, E., & Walker, E . (2005). Contributions
of music video exposure to black adolescents gender and
sexual schemas. Journal of Adolescent Research, 20, 143
166.
West, C., & Zimmerman, D. H. (1991). Doing gender. In J. Lorber & S.
A. Farrell (Eds.), The social construction of gender (pp. 1337).
Newbury Park: Sage.
Whitely, S. (2000). Women and popular music: Sexuality, identity and
subjectivity. London: Routledge.
Williams, F., & Monge, P. (2001). Reasoning with statistics: How to
read quantitative research (5th ed.). Fort Worth: Harcourt.
Zhang, Y., Miller, L. E., & Harrison, K. (2008). The relationship
between exposure to sexual music videos and young adults
sexual attitudes. Journal of Broadcasting and Electronic Media,
52, 368386.
172 Sex Roles (2011) 64:160172
View publication statsView publication stats