Ann Appl Sport Sci,
12(S1): e1279, 2024.
http://www.aassjournal.com; e-ISSN: 23224479; p-ISSN: 24764981. 10.61186/aassjournal.1279
*. Corresponding Author:
Sheiladevi Sukumaran, Ph.D.
ORIGINAL ARTICLE
Enhancing Tennis Performance through Visual Training: The Efficacy
of Dynamic Vision Exercises
1
Zhang Die Die ,
2
Sheiladevi Sukumaran
*
1
Liming Vocational University, Quanzhou City, Fujian Province, China.
2
Faculty of Education, Language,
Psychology and Music, SEGi University, Malaysia.
Submitted October 18, 2023; Accepted in final form December 28, 2023.
ABSTRACT
Background. This research examined the impact of visual training on tennis skill levels. Tennis demands high visual
capabilities from athletes, requiring them to maintain focus throughout the entire game to secure winning points and
ensure victory. The significant impact of visual training on enhancing tennis performance has not been thoroughly
investigated. Objectives. The purpose of this study is to investigate the impact of an 8-week visual training on tennis
performance and to identify the correlation between the effects of visual training and Tennis Performance. Methods.
Participants (n=50) engaged in dynamic vision exercises using The Sensory Station Training Application and practiced
tennis hitting while wearing dynamic vision training devices. The training program aimed to enhance participants'
visual skills in real tennis scenarios. The increase of visual training is an effective method to improve tennis skill levels.
Results. After eight weeks of visual training, the experimental group's ITN average score increased by 40.80 points,
indicating a significant improvement. The findings showed that there was no significant improvement in static visual
acuity, including visual clarity (Cohen's d=0.05) and perception span (Cohen's d=0.08). However, notable
improvements were observed in dynamic visual acuity, specifically in hand-eye coordination (Cohen's d=1.26),
reaction time (Cohen's d=1.71), and GO/NO GO (Cohen's d=3.11). Conclusion. These studies provide compelling
evidence supporting the idea that visual training can enhance tennis performance.
KEYWORDS: Dynamic Vision Exercises, Tennis Skills Levels, Training, Visual Training.
INTRODUCTION
Visual training was first initiated in France by
Javal and Manuel (1). Since then, the field of
ophthalmology has seen continuous development
and diversification of visual training methods
across generations of practitioners. It is now
widely applied in various fields, including sports
education, driving skills, professional skills,
academic learning, and performing arts. Visual
training has sparked significant interest among
researchers and coaches as a widely discussed and
applied method in various sports disciplines. The
concept of sports vision may vary among different
scholars. Sports vision is a specific visual ability
that differs from other visual functions. This ability
is crucial for athletes to process and perform at
their best. Athletes must gather a considerable
amount of visual information quickly. Evidence
shows that visual skills vary between athletes and
non-athletes, and impact athletic performance (2,
3). Research on sports vision is still in its early
stages, but it is widely recognized as a critical
component of athletic performance (4, 5). In
sports, vision is vital in providing athletes with
crucial information on where, when, and what to
do. Without the ability to quickly and accurately
process visual information, even the strongest,
2 Enhancing Tennis Performance through Visual Training
fastest, and most technically skilled athlete may
not reach their full potential. Dynamic visual skill
is mainly involved in catching or interceptive
actions of ball sports, whereas strategic sports use
different visual skills (peripheral and spatial
vision) due to the sport-specific requirements (6).
Sports vision is a specific visual ability that
differs from other visual functions. Sports Vision
is defined as the brain's ability to receive and
provide feedback on the messages transmitted by
the eyes. Sports vision involves the use of the
visual senses to acquire information about the
playing field (7). The brain processes this
information and generates instructions for the body
to execute specific actions (8).
Appelbaum & Erickson (2018) defined sports
vision as the brain's ability to receive and provide
feedback on the messages transmitted by the eyes.
Sports vision involves the use of the visual senses
to acquire information about the playing field. The
brain processes this information and generates
instructions for the body to execute specific actions
(9). Sports vision training can improve balance and
increase the fixation of Inline Hockey Players (10).
Visual training has emerged as a promising
technique to improve the visual skills of tennis
players. By engaging in repetitive eye exercises
prescribed by sports optometrists, tennis players
can enhance their basic visual functions, such as
visual acuity, depth perception, and peripheral
vision. These visual skills are crucial in tennis, as
they directly impact the athlete's ability to
accurately track the ball, anticipate its movement,
and make split-second decisions on how to
respond. The study by Şenol, Deniz, et al.
compared the visual reaction times (VRT) and
auditory reaction times (ART) of individuals with
different somatotypes, including athletes and
sedentary individuals. The findings indicated that
regular training and participation in sports were
associated with reduced VRT and ART (11).
The significance of sports vision differs from
one sport to another. Tennis, a popular sport
worldwide, has athletes and enthusiasts constantly
aiming for better performance. In tennis, the ball
moves swiftly in various directions during
gameplay. Athletes must rapidly assess the
trajectory and landing spot of the ball with
accuracy. Any approach that can enhance an
athlete's visual system will ultimately improve
their ability. To improve their technical abilities
and excel in competitive sports, athletes constantly
look for ways to enhance their skill sets. Athlete's
visual strategy in the game is related to eye
dynamics, not to head movements (12). In the case
of tennis, a dynamic sport with rapidly changing
ball speeds and directions, players must be able to
make quick and accurate judgments about the
trajectory and landing position of the ball.
Enhancing visual perception is crucial as it directly
impacts an athlete's ability to execute precise
movements swiftly. Afshar, Azam, Jaleh Baqerli,
et al. conducted a study to assess the impact of
visual training on the shooting skills of female
soccer players. They utilized Raven and Gibor's
vision exercises as their research tools and
administered a two-week visual training program
to the participants. The results indicated that the
sports-specific visual exercises led to an
enhancement in the players' shooting skills.
Consequently, any technique or method that can
optimize an athlete's visual system's functioning
will undoubtedly improve accuracy and speed in
executing skilled maneuvers.
Several studies involving baseball, table tennis,
and other sports revealed that excellent dynamic
vision was important in a variety of ball sports and
fast-reacting competitive sports, particularly open-
ended, multiplayer, and confrontational sports. For
example, Huang (13) discovered that visual scene
training greatly improved rookie tennis players'
visual search abilities, cognitive functions, and
specialized sports skills. In previous research, it
has been demonstrated that video training can
enhance the throwing speed of novice baseball
players and improve their ability to predict and
assess the trajectory of baseball (14). Additionally,
the implementation of dynamic vision training is
advantageous in enhancing athlete performance.
Moreover, intervention training focused on sports
vision was conducted with softball athletes,
leading to a relative improvement in their target
capture ability and execution of softball actions
(15, 16). Previous research has extensively
investigated the potential of visual training to
enhance athlete performance. Nevertheless, there
is a notable scarcity of research within the domain
of tennis sports education concerning the influence
of visual training on athletes' technical proficiency
and competitive capabilities.
MATERIALS AND METHODS
Methodology. This research aims to
investigate the impact of visual training on tennis
sports education. Experimental research by
organizing an experimental group and a control
Enhancing Tennis Performance through Visual Training 3
group, each undergoing different training
methods, as shown in Table 3, which is the tennis
skill training schedule. Data recorded for both
groups evaluate the effects of visual training on
improving tennis players' technical proficiency,
reaction speed, and performance in matches.
Through this research, new perspectives and
methods for sports teaching, thereby providing a
scientific basis for more significant advances and
achievements for athletes at the level of skills.
The results of several research scholars (12, 17)
confirm that visual training improves the
effectiveness of tennis sports teachings and
accurately improves the skill levels of tennis.
Individuals who underwent visual training
showed a higher degree of improvement in tennis
skill levels compared to those who did not receive
any training.
Participants. The research involved two
groups of students from the National Chinese
University College of Sports, specializing in
Tennis Education. Initially, 75 specialized
students underwent a basic vision test, and those
with short-sightedness or difficulty seeing distant
objects were excluded. Ultimately, 60 students
with naked eyesight greater than 1.0 were selected
for the research A simple random sampling
method was employed to select 60 student
subjects. The numbers 1 to 47 were assigned to
the 60 students, and each number was written on
a label. These labels were then mixed thoroughly,
and 50 labels were randomly drawn without
replacement. The students corresponding to the
numbers on these 50 labels formed the final
sample. The selected students were divided into
two groups: the control group, consisting of the
first 25 samples, and the experimental group,
consisting of the next 25 samples. Before starting
the experiment, the height and weight data of the
subjects were measured by the height and weight
meter, and the measurement verified that the BMI
of the students was in the normal range.
The premise of visual training is normal
vision. Before the experiment, an E-chart was
used to verify the subjects' normal vision.
According to Table 1, the participants'
background information comprises 25 students
from both the experimental and control groups.
The average age of the students in the
experimental group was 20.47±0.68, and the
average age in the control group was
20.54±0.45. The average height of the
experimental group was 173.75±5.59, and the
average height of the control group was
174.34±576. Body Mass Index (BMI) is a value
calculated by dividing a person's weight by the
square of their height. It primarily reflects the
overall body weight and is used as an indicator
to assess the individual's overall nutritional
status Yajnik and Yudkin (17). The average BMI
of the student in the experimental group was
18.25±1.15, and that of the control group was
17.85±1.85. Therefore, 50 students who
participated in the experimental research had
normal naked-eye vision, were in good physical
condition, and could engage in moderate
physical exercise activities. All participants in
this research volunteered to participate,
everyone signed the experimental notice and can
withdraw at any time.
Table 1. Basic Information of 50 Sample
Age
Height (CM)
Weight (KG)
BMI (W/H
2
)
Vision
Experimental
Group
20.47±0.68
173.75±5.59
55.39±7.18
18.25±1.15
1.0
Control
Group
20.54±0.45
174.34±5.76
54.87±9.15
17.85±1.85
1.0
Measures. Prior to starting the visual training,
a visual ability test was administered to 50
participants. During this test, the participants'
current levels of visual abilities were evaluated,
and the resulting data were recorded and stored to
gather the raw scores of their existing visual
abilities. To facilitate the visual skills exercises, the
participants engaged with the Sensory Station
Training Application. There were 9 system test
items (as depicted in Figure 1 and Table 3): The
utilization of this well-structured application
ensured a comprehensive and systematic approach
to evaluating and enhancing the participants' visual
capabilities. Firstly, visual clarity pertains to the
ability of the eyes to see objects clearly, serving as
the foundation for any visual task. Contrast
sensitivity marks the initiation of the visual process
and holds vital importance in recognizing objects
and faces. Near-far quickness evaluates the
capacity of individuals to swiftly adjust their focus,
4 Enhancing Tennis Performance through Visual Training
essential for tasks involving motion, spatial
judgment, and timely decision-making. Perception
span denotes the breadth of the visual field,
enabling the efficient gathering of diverse visual
information for quick and accurate processing.
Target capture assesses how rapidly one can
identify and focus on the most pertinent visual data
among available information. Multiple Object
Tracking allows individuals to track and navigate
a target, crucial for collision avoidance when
dealing with multiple moving objects. Reaction
time denotes the speed at which individuals
respond to visual stimuli, a critical aspect of
various activities requiring swift decision-making.
Eye-hand coordination measures the ability to
synchronize eye movement with hand movement,
determining the accuracy of executing actions after
receiving visual information and brain instructions.
In the GO/NO GO test, participants are required to
swiftly touch "Go" targets before they disappear
while avoiding "No-Go" targets, assessing quick
decision-making and rapid response skills.
To compare the differences between the two
groups of data, Cohen's d was chosen in this
research. The formula for effect size (Cohen's d) is
as follows:
Cohen's d=(M1-M2)/SD,
M1 and M2 represent the meaning of the two
sample groups, and SD was the average of the
standard deviations of the two sample groups.
Table 3 illustrates the statistical analysis of
diverse visual abilities within the experimental
and control groups. The calculation of effect
sizes for each test, employing Cohen's d formula,
provided insights into the distinctions between
the data sets of both groups. The effect sizes
observed for all nine visual ability tests in the
two groups were consistently below 0.8,
indicating relatively modest differences in the
data sets for each group. Hence, the findings
showed that there was a similarity in the levels
of visual abilities between the experimental and
control groups before the commencement of
visual training.
Figure 1. Assessment Items in the present study.
Procedures. Both the experimental and
control groups engaged in an eight-week training
program focusing on basic tennis skills. The
training sessions were conducted at the Chinese
University Tennis Course and encompassed
various tennis skill exercises, including
Forehand baseline stroke (FBS), Backhand
Baseline Stroke (BBS), Forehand volleyball
(FV), Backhand volley (BV), Mid-PS, Forecourt
pressure shot (Fore PS), First serve (FS), Second
serve (SS), and other techniques. The index
method test results are displayed in Table 2.
Enhancing Tennis Performance through Visual Training 5
Table 2. Testing Index and Methods
Test index
Test method
Visual
clarity
The participants held a mobile device while standing 3 meters away from a tablet computer for the test.
On the tablet computer, a black Landolt ring appears with a random missing section at the top, bottom,
left, or right, presented in a random sequence on a white background according to predefined acuity
requirements. The participants are required to identify the direction of the missing section and swipe the
mobile device in the corresponding direction.
Contrast
sensitivity
The participants stand 3 meters away from a tablet computer while holding a mobile device. On the screen,
four black circular shapes are displayed, with one of them containing concentric circles of varying shades.
The participants are required to identify the circular shape with the concentric circles and swipe the mobile
device in the corresponding direction.
Target
capture
The participants stand 3 meters away from the large screen while holding a mobile device. Randomly, a
Landolt ring will suddenly appear in one of the four corners of the large screen. The participants are
required to judge the gap direction of the Landolt ring and swipe the mobile device in the corresponding
direction on the mobile device's screen.
Near-far
quickness
The participants stand 3 meters away from a tablet computer while holding a mobile device positioned
40cm from their eyes. During the test, Landolt rings alternately appear on the tablet computer and the
mobile device screen. After determining the direction of the gap in the ring displayed on the tablet screen,
the participants immediately swipe the mobile device in the corresponding direction and quickly identify
the gap direction in the ring on the mobile device screen. They repeat this sequence, switching focus
between the distant and near screens within the 30s.
Multiple
object
tracking
The participants are positioned 60cm away from the tablet computer. On the screen, multiple sets of balls
are displayed, each consisting of two black balls, with one of them briefly turning red and then returning
to black. Subsequently, the balls in each set start rotating randomly in both clockwise and
counterclockwise directions for several rounds before coming to a stop. The participants are required to
identify the ball in each set that initially turned red.
Perception
span
The participants stand 60cm away from the tablet computer, with their eyes aligned horizontally with the
center of the screen. During the test, a certain number of circles appear on the screen. Initially, the circles
form a pattern radiating outward from the center. Some of these circles have a central black dot that appears
in a very brief period. After the black dot disappears, the participants need to identify the circle where the
black dot appeared and click on it.
Eye-hand
coordination
The participants stand 60cm away from the large screen. Randomly, 96 circular rings are displayed in 8
columns on the screen. At the start of the test, a random point appears within one of the circular rings,
and the participants must quickly and accurately click on this point. Once the point is touched, another
point appears at a random location. The goal is to click on as many points as possible within a specified
time. The higher the number of correct clicks, the higher the test score.
GO/NO GO
The participants stand 60cm away from the large screen. On the screen, 96 circular shapes are displayed
in 8 columns, each designed to mimic eye-hand coordination. Within these circular shapes, green or red
dots appear pseudo-randomly. When a green dot appears, the participants need to quickly tap it, and when
a red dot appears, they must refrain from tapping it.
Reaction
time
The participants stand 60cm away from the tablet, with their eye level aligned with the center of the screen.
At the start of the test, two circular shapes appear on the screen. Using their index fingers on both hands,
they lightly touch the center of each circle. Once their fingers are correctly aligned with the centers, the
circular shapes change color to green. Subsequently, the two circular shapes randomly turn red, and the
corresponding index fingers must be swiftly lifted and then pressed down again. The faster this action is
performed, the higher the score.
Table 3. Visual Ability Level of Participants
Experimental
Group
Control
Group
Cohen's d
Visual clarity
17.18±1.32
18.22±1.54
-0.68
Contrast sensitivity
30.07±1.16
29.78±1.02
0.29
Near-far quickness
19.33±3.14
20.46±2.98
-0.37
Perception span
58.94±4.87
59.59±6.12
0.11
Target capture
20.94±1.28
21.19±0.76
0.22
Multiple object tracking
70.82±2.25
69.83±3.87
0.31
Reaction time
14.18±3.61
15.22±2.65
0.29
Eye-hand coordination
50.80±4.87
49.81±4.75
0.21
GO/NO GO
29.28±4.91
30.87±5.27
0.32
6 Enhancing Tennis Performance through Visual Training
The tennis basic skills exercises were carried
out three times a week, with each session lasting
for 90 minutes. To ensure the participant's safety
and well-being, warming-up and relaxation
activities were incorporated at the beginning and
end of each training session to prevent
unnecessary physical injuries. The detailed
training schedule is provided in Table 4.
Table 4 outlines the schedule of skills
exercises to be carried out by all participants in
the research The exercises have been designed to
incorporate a diverse range of techniques,
providing athletes with comprehensive practice
opportunities for each skill. Notably, the
experimental group was required to wear dynamic
vision glasses (Model 2MJ03SE-868, also known
as the Primary Dong Dynamic Vision Trainer).
The purpose of using these glasses was twofold:
first, to heighten the difficulty level for the
experimental group in receiving visual
information, and second, to reduce the frequency
of interruptions in the dynamic visual trainer
blocking the picture surface. As a result, the path
to the ball becomes more inconsistent,
necessitating the experimental group of students
to focus their attention entirely on gathering
visual information. Subsequently, they must
transmit this information to the brain and execute
the corresponding action while practicing their
tennis skills.
Table 4. Schedule of Skills Exercises
Week
Day
Training
Week 1
Monday 2 pm-3:30 pm
FBS+BBS, FS
Wednesday 4 pm-5:30 pm
Mid-PS+ Fore PS, SS
Friday 4 pm-5:30 pm
FV+BV, SS, Footwork training
Week 2
Monday 2 pm-3:30 pm
Mid-PS+ Fore PS, FS
Wednesday 4 pm-5:30 pm
FV+BV, SS
Friday 4 pm-5:30 pm
FBS+BBS, FS, Footwork training
Week 3
Monday 2 pm-3:30 pm
FBS+BBS, FS
Wednesday 4 pm-5:30 pm
Mid-PS+ Fore PS, SS
Friday 4 pm-5:30 pm
FV+BV, SS
Week 4
Monday 2 pm-3:30 pm
Mid-PS+ Fore PS, FS
Wednesday 4 pm-5:30 pm
FV+BV, SS
Friday 4 pm-5:30 pm
FBS+BBS, FS
Week 5
Monday 2 pm-3:30 pm
FBS+BBS, FS, Footwork training
Wednesday 4 pm-5:30 pm
Mid-PS+ Fore PS, SS
Friday 4 pm-5:30 pm
FV+BV, SS
Week 6
Monday 2 pm-3:30 pm
Mid-PS+ Fore PS, FS, Footwork training
Wednesday 4 pm-5:30 pm
FV+BV, SS
Friday 4 pm-5:30 pm
FBS+BBS, FS
Week 7
Monday 2 pm-3:30 pm
FBS+BBS, FS
Wednesday 4 pm-5:30 pm
Mid-PS+ Fore PS, SS
Friday 4 pm-5:30 pm
FV+BV, SS
Week 8
Monday 2 pm-3:30 pm
Mid-PS+ Fore PS, FS
Wednesday 4 pm-5:30 pm
FV+BV, SS, Footwork training
Friday 4 pm-5:30 pm
FBS+BBS, FS
As illustrated in Figure 2, a Feeder (F) was
stationed in the field, handing the ball to the
opposing Player (P) and conducting skills
exercises according to the predetermined
schedule. The primary objective of this exercise
was to ensure that all participants engaged in
tennis skills exercises with an equal amount of
practice and intensity.
The experimental team, in addition to wearing
dynamic vision glasses during tennis ball
practice, also engaged in auxiliary visual skills
exercises utilizing the ZF-Test Attention Capacity
Test. Following the specialized tennis training,
participants moved to the indoor classroom at the
tennis court for attention span training, with each
session lasting 90 seconds per person and was
conducted in two consecutive sessions.
The ZF-test was utilized as a focus range
measurement method and device in this research.
It involved placing multiple LED light keys on a
test panel, arranged in a composition matrix (as
shown in Figure 3). Testers initiated the test by
pressing the start button to initiate the timing.
During the test, when an LED light on the test
board was illuminated, another LED light
immediately lit up simultaneously. If a tester
Enhancing Tennis Performance through Visual Training 7
failed to press the illuminated light within the
predetermined interval time, this instance was not
recorded in the score, and the light switched to
another LED light. This process continued until
the timing was completed. Finally, the test score
was displayed and recorded for subsequent
analysis.
The purpose of this exercise was to enhance
the stability, concentration, and flexibility of
visual attention. Testers were required to
concentrate their attention on specific targets and
quickly switch their focus to other targets, to
improve their perception and responsiveness to
the surrounding environment.
Figure 2. Position of Feeder and Player.
Figure 3. ZF-Test Panel Plot.
Analysis. To assess the students' specialized
tennis skills, this research utilized the International
Tennis Number (ITN) as a measurement tool and
incorporated the collected data into the overall
analysis. The normality distribution was confirmed
by inspecting Skewness, Kurtosis, Kolmogorov-
Smirnov statistics, and Shapiro-Wilk statistics.
The International Tennis Number (ITN) was used
as a measurement instrument in this research to
assess the students' specialized tennis skills, and
the obtained data was incorporated into the overall
analysis. Skewness, Kurtosis, Kolmogorov-
Smirnov, and Shapiro-Wilk statistics were used to
confirm the normality of the distribution. The
visual ability and ITN scores have Kolmogorov-
Smirnov values greater than 0.05. The Z-score for
skewness was calculated as 0.078/0.141=0.55, and
the Z-score for Kurtosis was 0.262/0.281=0.93.
8 Enhancing Tennis Performance through Visual Training
The skewness and kurtosis values were close to
zero, and the Z-scores were within 1.96 standard
deviations of the mean. This showed that the data
gathered in this research for visual ability scores
and ITN scores has a normal distribution.
RESULTS
International Tennis Number (ITN). The
purpose of this research was to explore the impact
of visual training on tennis sports education. To
achieve this, the International Tennis Number
(ITN) was used to quantify the level of tennis
skills among the participants, and the student skill
scores of both the experimental and control
groups were statistically summarized.
Table 5 shows that both groups underwent
statistical data analysis before and after the
experiment. The control group had an average ITN
score of 99.28 before training, with a total score of
2482.00 and a range of 21. After training, the
control group had an average ITN score of 106.76,
with a total score of 2669.00 and a range of 29. The
paired-sample t-test for the control group showed
a value of -7.067 with a significance level of 0.000.
For the experimental group, the average ITN score
before training was 98.48, with a total score of
2462.00 and a range of 21. After training, the
average ITN score increased to 139.28, with a total
score of 3482.00 and a range of 42. The paired
sample t-test for the experimental group showed a
value of -28.199 with a significance level of 0. 000.
After eight weeks of visual training, the
experimental group's average score increased by
40.80 points, significantly improved.
Table 5. ITN of 25 samples
Figure 4 displays a line chart illustrating the
changes in tennis skill levels before and after
visual learning for both the experimental group
and the control group, each consisting of 50
samples.
The Sensory Station Training Application.
The research quantified the participants' tennis
skill levels using the International Tennis Number
(ITN). Subsequently, the skill scores of students
in both the experimental and control groups were
Control
Group
Pro-test
ITN
Post-test
ITN
Effect
Size
Experimental
Group
Pro-test
ITN
Post-test
ITN
Effect
Size
A1
94
97
3
B1
104
139
35
A2
111
120
9
B2
93
129
36
A3
96
104
8
B3
97
135
38
A4
105
112
7
B4
95
135
40
A5
95
100
5
B5
111
166
55
A6
93
103
10
B6
98
130
32
A7
106
101
-5
B7
106
139
33
A8
101
107
6
B8
104
141
37
A9
90
94
4
B9
93
135
42
A10
97
106
9
B10
96
147
51
A11
104
115
11
B11
98
148
50
A12
100
103
3
B12
92
141
49
A13
93
101
8
B13
95
131
36
A14
95
112
7
B14
91
134
43
A15
111
110
-1
B15
98
137
39
A16
98
108
10
B16
102
142
40
A17
95
96
1
B17
110
146
36
A18
92
99
7
B18
95
126
31
A19
108
114
6
B19
94
124
30
A20
102
111
9
B20
97
135
38
A21
106
123
7
B21
93
146
53
A22
90
101
11
B22
90
135
45
A23
103
121
9
B23
99
135
36
A24
100
108
8
B24
108
152
44
A25
97
103
6
B25
103
154
51
Mean
99.28
106.76
Mean
98.48
139.28
Total
2482.00
2669.00
Total
2462.00
3482.00
Range
21
29
Range
21
42
Enhancing Tennis Performance through Visual Training 9
subjected to thorough statistical analysis and
summarization.
Table 6 presented the Cohen's d values for
different visual abilities, with Visual clarity,
Contrast sensitivity, Perception span, and
multiple object tracking showed relatively small
Cohen's d values, while the remaining four
indicators exhibit relatively large Cohen's d
values. The assessment of visual abilities
encompassed both dynamic visual acuity and
static visual acuity. The findings from the training
conducted using The Sensory Station Training
Application revealed that there was no significant
improvement in static visual acuity, including
visual clarity (Cohen's d=0.05) and perception
span (Cohen's d=0.08). However, notable
improvements were observed in dynamic visual
acuity, specifically in hand-eye coordination
(Cohen's d=1.26), reaction time (Cohen's d=1.71)
and GO/NO GO (Cohen's d=3.11).
Figure 4. Changes in Tennis Skill Levels.
Table 6. Visual Ability Level of Pre-Post
`
Experimental
Group Pre-Test
Experimental
Group Post-Test
Cohen's d
Visual clarity
17.18±1.32
17.12±1.92
0.05
Contrast sensitivity
30.07±1.16
30.13±2.06
-0.035
Near-far quickness
19.33±3.14
25.67±3.60
2.03
Perception span
58.94±4.87
59.32±5.05
0.08
Target capture
20.94±1.28
21.16±1.70
0.13
Multiple object tracking
70.82±2.25
72.20±2.85
0.48
Reaction time
14.18±3.61
20.80±4.26
1.71
Eye-hand coordination
50.80±4.87
57.12±5.08
1.26
GO/NO GO
29.28±4.91
44.68±5.15
3.11
10 Enhancing Tennis Performance through Visual Training
DISCUSSION
The objective of this research was to examine
the influence of visual training on tennis physical
education and its potential to enhance tennis skill
levels. Moreover, the researchers sought to
introduce a straightforward and practical visual
training method into university sports courses.
During the 8-week study, the experimental group
students underwent visual ability training,
resulting in notable improvements in their visual
abilities. Additionally, the enhanced tennis skill
levels observed in this group provided further
evidence of the effectiveness of utilizing the
dynamic visual training device.
The visual abilities, including static and dynamic
visual acuity showed different levels of
improvement after the training. In this research
confirmed that when visual training was applied
with high-ball speed and high-focus tasks, athletes
demonstrated significant improvements in tracking
ball trajectories and making precise ball predictions.
Before research, several scholars have obtained
consistent research results in various sports. Visual
training has been shown to significantly enhance
athletic performance in a range of sports, such as
badminton, baseball, table tennis, and football.
Before this research, several studies have been
conducted to investigate the effects of visual
training on athletic performance across these sports.
For instance, Feng (18) examined its impact on
badminton players, while Liu et al. (19) and Millard
(20) focused on baseball players, and Coetzee and
de Waal (21) focused on netball players. Similarly,
Shinkai et al. (10) researched table tennis, and Yang
et al. (22) investigated its applications in football.
Skeet Shooting Athletes have better hand-eye
coordination and visual memory than non-athletes
(23). Collectively, these studies provide compelling
evidence supporting the notion that visual training
can yield significant improvements in athletic
performance in a diverse range of sports. Although
research results demonstrate that visual training can
enhance fundamental tennis skills, the specific skills
and techniques with the most significant
improvement remain unclear.
The continuous advancements in the ability of
visual training devices have raised intriguing
questions about their potential impact on athletes'
physiological structure and subsequent sports
performance. As this critical question remains
unanswered, it was strongly recommended further
research in this field to delve deeper into these
aspects. By investigating the effects of visual training
on athletes' performance, the research adds to the
growing interest in understanding the training method
can contribute to improved sports proficiency.
CONCLUSION
The findings of this study will not only provide
valuable insight and recommendations for athletes
and coaches but will also contribute to the
advancement of tennis education and the
improvement of athletes' competitiveness in the
sport. Furthermore, as technology continues to
evolve and visual training devices become
increasingly popular, the study highlights the
importance of delving deeper into their potential
impact on the physiological aspects of athletes.
Addressing this gap in the research field will help to
better understand the mechanisms by which visual
training enhances sports performance, ultimately
opening up new possibilities for optimizing sports
training and performance in the future.
From a theoretical perspective, this research
can be grounded in the framework of perceptual
motor skills and neural adaptations. Visual
training is thought to improve athletes' perceptual
skills, including the perception of movement
trajectory, speed, and position, which is crucial
for making quick decisions in fast-paced sports
such as tennis. Additionally, neuroadaptive
theory suggests that consistent training and
practice can improve the way the brain processes
specific motor tasks, leading to improved athletic
performance. By examining how visual training
affects perceptual-motor skills and neural
adaptations, a fuller understanding of its impact
on athletes' competitive abilities can be gained.
In conclusion, this study will not only provide
practical advice for athletes and coaches but also
provide theoretical support for the development
of tennis education. In addition, by thoroughly
investigating the physiological effects of visual
training, we can reveal its potential mechanisms
for optimizing sports training and performance
and provide new directions for future sports
science research and sports training.
APPLICABLE REMARKS
The primary aim of this study was to examine
the impact of visual training on tennis physical
education and skill enhancement, focusing on
the introduction of a practical visual training
method into university sports courses.
The experimental group of students who
underwent visual ability training during 8 weeks
Enhancing Tennis Performance through Visual Training 11
demonstrated substantial improvements in their
visual abilities, including static and dynamic
visual acuity.
The study's results provided evidence that
utilizing a dynamic visual training device was
effective in enhancing tennis skill levels,
particularly when high ball speed and high-
focus tasks were employed.
This research contextualized its findings by
referring to prior studies in various sports like
badminton, baseball, table tennis, and football, all
of which supported the idea that visual training
can significantly improve athletic performance.
The study also acknowledged the need for
further investigation to determine which
specific skills and techniques benefited the
most from this training.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
In preparing this academic manuscript, we
wish to clarify that we have not received financial
backing from any external organization, and there
exists no affiliations or associations that could
potentially influence the content or findings of
this paper.
AUTHORS’ CONTRIBUTIONS
Study concept and design: Zhang Die Die,
Sheiladevi Sukumaran. Acquisition of data:
Sheiladevi Sukumaran. Analysis and interpretation
of data: Zhang Die Die. Drafting the manuscript:
Zhang Die Die, Sheiladevi Sukumaran. Critical
revision of the manuscript for important intellectual
content: Sheiladevi Sukumaran. Statistical analysis:
Sheiladevi Sukumaran. Administrative, technical, and
material support: Zhang Die Die. Study supervision:
Sheiladevi Sukumaran.
CONFLICT OF INTEREST
The authors declare that there are no conflicts
of interest that could have inappropriately.
REFERENCES
1. Javal É. Manuel théorique et pratique du strabisme: G. Masson; 1896.
2. Sánchez-Tena MÁ, Rodríguez-Alonso X, Martinez-Perez C, Tornero-Aguilera JF, Clemente-Suárez VJ,
Sanchez-Ramos C, et al. Comparison of Visual Skills between Federated and Non-Federated Athletes.
International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health [Internet]. 2023; 20(2).
[doi:10.3390/ijerph20021047] [PMid:36673806]
3. Sakti IW. The analysis factors of experential marketing, product quality, and customer satisfaction of
motor bike as a main transportation mode in Bandung-Indonesia. International Journal of Business and
Administrative Studies. 2016;2(1):6-8. [doi:10.20469/ijbas.2.10002-1]
4. Lee JS, Liu YH, Chen WM, Lin KK, Chang ST, Lim AY, et al. Association of sports vision with age,
gender, and static visual acuity among nonathletic population. Taiwan J Ophthalmol. 2022;12(1):53-60.
[doi:10.4103/tjo.tjo_60_20] [PMid:35399972]
5. Narimani M, Mikaeil N. The Effectiveness of Cognitive-Behavioral Therapy in Decreasing Depression
and Increasing Happiness and Life Satisfaction. Opción: Revista de Ciencias Humanas y Sociales.
2018(17):1182-97.
6. Presta V, Vitale C, Ambrosini L, Gobbi G. Stereopsis in Sports: Visual Skills and Visuomotor Integration
Models in Professional and Non-Professional Athletes. International Journal of Environmental Research
and Public Health [Internet]. 2021; 18(21). [doi:10.3390/ijerph182111281] [PMid:34769799]
7. Appelbaum LG, Erickson G. Sports vision training: A review of the state-of-the-art in digital training
techniques. International Review of Sport and Exercise Psychology. 2018;11(1):160-89.
[doi:10.1080/1750984X.2016.1266376]
8. Wu Hongyu, Peng Cilong, Ling. Y, editors. A Discussion on Sports Vision. In Proceedings of the 2011
Second International Conference on Education and Sports Education (ESE 2011 V5); 2011: Intelligent
Information Technology Application Association.
9. Heydari H, Badami R, Meshkati Z. The effect of sports vision training on cognitive skills in inline hockey
players. Razi Journal of Medical Sciences.0-.
10. Shinkai R, Ando S, Nonaka Y, Kizuka T, Ono S. Visual Strategies for Eye and Head Movements During
Table Tennis Rallies. Front Sports Act Living. 2022;4:897373. [doi:10.3389/fspor.2022.897373]
[PMid:35655529]
11. Al-Dmour BA, Al-Nawayseh A-HT, Al-Tarawneh MA, Hani ZMB. Antecedents of Mental Disorder
among Physically Inactive Employees Study of Jordanian Higher Education Institutions: Mediated
12 Enhancing Tennis Performance through Visual Training
Moderation of Perceived Threat of Covid-19 and Psychological Capital. Pakistan Journal of Life &
Social Sciences. 2023;21(1). [doi:10.57239/PJLSS-2023-21.1.001]
12. Wang C, Yan A, Deng W, Qi C. Effect of Tennis Expertise on Motion-in-Depth Perception at Different
Speeds: An Event-Related Potential Study. Brain Sciences [Internet]. 2022; 12(9).
[doi:10.3390/brainsci12091160] [PMid:36138895]
13. Huang HY. Experimental Study on the Effectiveness of Specialized Perceptual Skill Training for Tennis
Players. Journal of Tianjin Institute of Physical Education. 2013;1:52-5.
14. Ranganathan R, Carlton LG. Perception-action coupling and anticipatory performance in baseball
batting. J Mot Behav. 2007;39(5):369-80. [doi:10.3200/JMBR.39.5.369-380] [PMid:17827114]
15. Appelbaum LG, Lu Y, Khanna R, Detwiler KR. The effects of sports vision training on sensorimotor
abilities in collegiate softball athletes. Athletic Training & Sports Health Care. 2016;8(4):154-63.
[doi:10.3928/19425864-20160314-01]
16. Moschogianni G. Combined effects of self tuning model and innovation Capability on digital innovation:
A study of German pharmaceutical firm. Journal of Digitovation and information system. 2021;1(1):15-
27. [doi:10.54433/JDIIS.2021100002]
17. Yajnik CS, Yudkin JS. The Y-Y paradox. Lancet. 2004;363(9403):163. [doi:10.1016/S0140-
6736(03)15269-5] [PMid:14726172]
18. Feng. Discussion on Speed Training Method in Youth Badminton Teaching. Advances in Physical
Sciences. 2021;9(1):10812. [doi:10.12677/APS.2021.91015]
19. Liu S, Ferris LM, Hilbig S, Asamoa E, LaRue JL, Lyon D, et al. Dynamic vision training transfers
positively to batting practice performance among collegiate baseball batters. Psychology of Sport and
Exercise. 2020;51:101759. [doi:10.1016/j.psychsport.2020.101759]
20. Millard L, Breukelman GJ, Mathe N, Shaw I, Shaw BS. A review of the essential visual skills required
for soccer: Beyond 20-20 optometry. Front Sports Act Living. 2022;4:965195.
[doi:10.3389/fspor.2022.965195] [PMid:36311213]
21. Coetzee D, de Waal E. An Exploratory Investigation of the Effect of a Sports Vision Program on Grade
4 and 5 Female Netball Players’ Visual Skills. International Journal of Environmental Research
and Public Health [Internet]. 2022; 19(16). [doi:10.3390/ijerph19169864] [PMid:36011502]
22. Yang J, Jing Y, Lan L. A study on the application of visual training in campus football training. Journal
of Physical Fitness Science. 2021;1(1):152.
23. Nascimento H, Alvarez-Peregrina C, Martinez-Perez C, Sánchez-Tena MÁ. Differences in Visuospatial
Expertise between Skeet Shooting Athletes and Non-Athletes. International Journal of Environmental
Research and Public Health [Internet]. 2021; 18(15). [doi:10.3390/ijerph18158147] [PMid:34360440]