East Tennessee State University East Tennessee State University
Digital Commons @ East Digital Commons @ East
Tennessee State University Tennessee State University
Electronic Theses and Dissertations Student Works
5-2005
Family Violence and Divorce: Effects on Marriage Expectations. Family Violence and Divorce: Effects on Marriage Expectations.
Hollie Nicole Dillon
East Tennessee State University
Follow this and additional works at: https://dc.etsu.edu/etd
Part of the Family, Life Course, and Society Commons, and the Psychology Commons
Recommended Citation Recommended Citation
Dillon, Hollie Nicole, "Family Violence and Divorce: Effects on Marriage Expectations." (2005).
Electronic
Theses and Dissertations.
Paper 998. https://dc.etsu.edu/etd/998
This Thesis - unrestricted is brought to you for free and open access by the Student Works at Digital Commons @
East Tennessee State University. It has been accepted for inclusion in Electronic Theses and Dissertations by an
authorized administrator of Digital Commons @ East Tennessee State University. For more information, please
Family Violence and Divorce:
Effects on Marriage Expectations
A thesis
presented to
the faculty of the Department of Psychology
East Tennessee State University
In partial fulfillment
of the requirements for the degree
Master of Arts in Clinical Psychology
by
Hollie N. Dillon
May 2005
Dr. Peggy Cantrell, Chair
Dr. Wallace Dixon
Dr. Jon Ellis
Keywords: Family Violence, Divorce, Marital Expectations
2
ABSTRACT
Family Violence and Divorce:
Effects on Marriage Expectations
by
Hollie N. Dillon
Family violence and divorce can have influential effects on marital expectations. The present
study analyzes the effects of gender, family violence, and divorce on marital expectations.
Participants were 293 students enrolled in an introductory psychology course at a southeastern
university. The mean age of the participants was 19.67 with 62.5% being female and 37.5%
being male. Participants completed the Conflict Tactics Scale to assess the presence of violence
in the participant’s family of origin. Participants were also assessed on their parents’ marital
status and, if applicable, their age at the time of divorce. This information was gathered via a
demographic questionnaire in order to group participants into intact and non-intact groups based
on their family of origin data. Participants also completed the Marriage Expectation Scale,
which assesses future marital expectations by mean scores. A participant’s score may indicate
pessimistic, realistic, or idealistic marital expectations. Results did not indicate that family
violence or divorce had an effect on the participant’s marital expectations. Results did indicate
statistically significant findings that revealed that men and women differed on marital
expectations. Critiques of the present study as well as implications for future research are
discussed.
3
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
First, I want to thank my thesis committee chairperson, Dr. Peggy Cantrell, for the help
you provided over the course of my thesis development. Thank you for walking ahead of me
when your vision was the livelihood of this project. Thank you for walking behind me when it
was time for me to rely on myself to make this project one I could be proud to call my own.
Most of all, thank you for walking beside me during the final stages when I could reflect on a
long and challenging road that had to come to an end.
Thank you, Dr. Dixon, for your high expectations and encouragement, which allowed me
to have higher expectations and be more encouraging of myself. Dr. Ellis, thank you for your
quiet encouragement and confidence in my abilities to complete this thesis.
To Adam, thank you for giving me the heart to persevere when I did not think I could
succeed. To Mom and Mike, thank you for your unconditional support that I have always known
I can count on. To Dad and Susan, thank you for exuding such confidence in me that pushed me
to finish, not somehow but triumphantly!
4
CONTENTS
Page
ABSTRACT .......................................................................................................................... 2
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS ..................................................................................................... 3
Chapter
1. INTRODUCTION ........................................................................................................ 6
Pertinent Theoretical Concepts ................................................................................. 8
Gender Role Theory ............................................................................................ 8
Social Learning Theory ....................................................................................... 14
Family Systems Theory ....................................................................................... 17
Empirical Findings..................................................................................................... 20
Importance of Conflict......................................................................................... 20
Divorce................................................................................................................. 22
Marital Expectations ........................................................................................... 22
Statement of the Problem........................................................................................... 24
Hypotheses........................................................................................................... 24
Design I.......................................................................................................... 24
Design II ........................................................................................................ 25
2. METHODS .................................................................................................................... 26
Participants ................................................................................................................ 26
Measures .................................................................................................................... 26
Procedure ................................................................................................................... 28
5
Chapter Page
Research Design ........................................................................................................ 28
Design I................................................................................................................ 28
Design II .............................................................................................................. 29
3. RESULTS ...................................................................................................................... 30
4. DISCUSSION ................................................................................................................ 34
REFERENCES ...................................................................................................................... 37
APPENDICES ....................................................................................................................... 39
Appendix A: Demographic Questionnaire ................................................................ 39
Appendix B: Conflict Tactics Scales (CTS) ............................................................. 40
Appendix C: Marriage Expectation Scale (MES)...................................................... 41
VITA ..................................................................................................................................... 43
6
CHAPTER 1
INTRODUCTION
In 2001, the National Center for Health Statistics reported that 2,254,000 marriages took
place. The marriage rate for that year was 7.8 per 1,000 of the total population. The divorce rate
was 4.0 per 1,000 of the total population. Therefore, divorces took place at more than half the
rate of marriages. In contrast, the divorce rate in 1950 was 2.6 per 1, 000 of the total population.
U.S. divorce statistics indicate that 1 million children are involved in the divorce of their parents
each year (DivorceMagazine.com, 2000). This article also reported that, as of 1998, 20 million
children under the age of 18 lived with only 1 parent. Further, it was reported that these children
of divorce are 50% more likely to divorce than children from intact families. Thus, these
statistics reflect the growing problem of divorce in the last 50 years for the American family as
well as the children of divorce.
The high divorce rate in America has been connected to marital dissatisfaction as a result
of unrealistic marital expectations (Larson, 1988). Research must look deeper into the effects on
children caused by the event of divorce. An article on the effects of divorce on America reports
the physical, emotional, and financial consequences of divorce on children (Fagan & Rector,
2000, ¶1). The physical consequences for children include an increased risk for being exposed
to violence and health problems. The emotional consequences comprise behavioral problems as
well as a higher risk for committing suicide. The financial effects of divorce force many
children to experience poverty after the divorce of their parents as a result of a 50% income drop.
Further, children whose parents have divorced do not perform as well academically and are at a
higher risk for dropping out of school. The broad range of effects children experience as a result
of divorce is not concentrated only in childhood and may carry on into adulthood.
Jones and Nelson (1996) reported that even though children of divorce had a negative
experience in relation to their own parents’ marriage, they still believe they can achieve a
7
positive marital experience. As a result, children of divorce may refuse to acknowledge the
characteristics that make marriage work (Jones & Nelson). Thus, idealistic marital expectations
may result. Wallerstein (1987) reported that children of divorce may continue to believe in
romantic love even 10 years after their parents have divorced. Further, Larson (1988) claimed
that Americans believe their wife or husband will concurrently perform a variety of roles, such
as lover, friend, parent, and counselor. When these idealistic marital expectations are not met,
marital dissatisfactions may take place and divorce may occur. Interestingly, children of divorce
display pessimistic marital expectations if marital violence and/or conflict is present in their
parents’ marriage (Grych & Fincham, 1990; Jones & Nelson; Markland & Nelson, 1993).
It is not only the event of divorce that causes negative consequences for the children of
divorce but also marital conflict and/or violence that may affect the reaction experienced by
children involved in divorce. Data from the National Family Violence surveys in 1975 and 1985
reflect that each year over 10 million children in America witness a physically violent act
between their parents (Straus, 1992). For two thirds of these children, it is not an isolated
occurrence of violence. The actual occurrence of this violence has been estimated at 3 times the
reported amount (Straus, 1992). The possible effects witnessing violence has on children are
numerous and may include future marital conflict and future marital violence that may lead to
divorce (Straus, 1992).
The quality of the family environment has proven to be an important factor in the identity
development of young adults (Markland & Nelson, 1993). It appears that the family structure is
not the only factor which may negatively affect the future adjustment of young adults. In the
past, divorce was considered to have the most harmful effects for the children involved in
relation to future adjustment. Research has supported the presence of family conflict as a factor
that may be just as important, if not more important than the negative effects caused by the event
of divorce (Markland & Nelson). Even if a family’s structure is intact, the presence of familial
conflict results in reports of a significantly lower self-concept by young adults in the areas of
8
confidence, sexual identity, and body concept (Markland & Nelson). Further, exposure to
familial conflict may cause pessimistic marital expectations to develop for these young adults,
which may damage interpersonal relationships later in life.
In conclusion, research has shown that the pattern of marital stability in American society
has been weakened as a result of divorce and marital violence and/or conflict. Children of
divorce may be greatly affected in terms of marital expectations they carry into their own
marriages. They may exhibit unrealistic marital expectations due to the belief that their spouse
will perform many roles simultaneously. On the other hand, children of divorce may exhibit
idealistic marital expectations due to the belief that a positive marital experience is easy to
achieve despite observing their own parents’ marital discord.
Last, children of divorce may exhibit pessimistic marital expectations as a result of
witnessing marital violence and/or conflict in their own parents’ marriage. It is important to look
further into the effects of divorce and family-of-origin conflict and/or violence on marital
expectations for the purpose of improving the state of marital stability in America. In addition,
children who have been involved in their parents’ divorce and/or witnessed their parents’ marital
violence and/or conflict need to be equipped with the skills to develop realistic marital
expectations.
Pertinent Theoretical Concepts
Gender Role Theory
In virtually all cultures, individuals are socialized to perform various roles based on
gender. Gender roles can be defined as intentions created by society for male or female behavior
(Lipman-Blumen, 1984). Socialization can be defined as a “set of mechanisms and processes
through which society trains its members to take their place as full-fledged social beings” (p.
53). Socialization explains how gender roles are learned through “describing the fundamental
processes by which individuals develop attitudes, expectations, behaviors, values, and skills that
9
coalesce into roles” (p. 53). Learning gender roles involves arranged and unplanned experiences
that promote continuation of behaving in a socially approved role. Socialization representatives
such as parents, teachers, peers, and public figures use specific and practical examples of gender
roles in order to socialize others. The socialization to perform various roles based on gender
begins from birth.
The event of birth for boys into the “blue world” as described by Lipman-Blumen (1984)
is symbolized by a blue blanket that marks the beginning of socialization into a prescribed
gender role. The goal for boys is to achieve masculinity. The “blue world” is characterized by
ideas that encourage boys to directly confront the environment and to take the world by storm.
Lipman-Blumen suggests that independent and direct action by way of force, power,
competition, and aggression are how boys should meet goals and accomplishments. Again,
direct action is emphasized in achievement through measuring discipline, supplies, and
outcomes. The male role demands independence, control of oneself, and control over others,
especially females (Lipman-Blumen).
On the other hand, Lipman-Blumen (1984) describes girls as being born into the “gentle
pink world,” which is symbolized by a pink blanket. As soon as girls are born, they are treated
as much more fragile than boys. For girls to achieve their prescribed gender role, femininity is
the goal. The “gentle pink world” is characterized by girls being required to stay closer to home
to assure they will be kept from physical danger. Clothing that is tailored to a delicate “little
lady” also suppresses their ability to play in a physically active manner. Girls staying close to
home also can be connected to their household duties that are assigned at young ages. Various
messages are sent to girls, such as, “Be a little lady,” and encourage involvement in household
duties.
Toys are another device through which children are taught gender roles. Advertising in
the toy industry usually pairs the appropriate gender-typed toy to a girl or boy (Richmond-
Abbott, 1992). As a result, society socializes its members to associate certain toys to be played
10
with by either girls or boys. It is often found that parents will encourage play with a gender-
typed toy that is appropriate for their child. Specific toys that are gender typed often encourage a
certain kind of play that is embedded in either a male gender role or a female gender role.
Richmond-Abbott (1992) stated, “Girls are more likely to get passive toys such as dish
sets, clothes, and coloring books” (p. 72). In addition, girls more often receive toys such as
sewing machines, ovens, dolls, dollhouses, and irons, which teach domestic activities that are
presumed to be performed later in life. After girls grow out of their “baby” dolls, “Barbie” dolls
are introduced as a model for how girls should look. Even though “Barbie” dolls have
progressed in diversity and range of activities that are considered appropriate, the dolls still
promote certain characteristics that are training tools for gender roles.
On the other hand, “Boys are more likely to get toys that encourage creativity and
manipulation: chemistry sets, erector sets, toy trains, and tool chests” (Richmond-Abbott, 1992,
p. 72). Action toys, mobile toys, and sports equipment encourage careers that require similar
interests. Boys have their own form of dolls, such as a “GI Joe” or a fighting character, that
cater to characteristics that boys are supposed to possess. Generally, boys are socialized to
display excitement and toughness through their toys.
In addition, “Even unisex equipment, such as bicycles, is differentiated in style, price,
and use between boys and girls” (Richmond-Abbott, 1992, p. 73). For example, a girl who
receives a pink “Barbie” bicycle will receive the message that she is to be a “little lady” when
riding her bike. On the other hand, a boy who receives a red “Hotwheels” bike will receive the
message that he is to be tough and masculine when riding his bike. Games are another example
where girls and boys receive messages about their gender roles.
The nature of play in games for girls is strikingly different from boys. Girls most often
play in small groups from childhood to the teenage years (Lipman-Blumen, 1984). Girls will
engage in play that is characterized by low structure and high cooperation (Richmond-Abbott,
1992). Early on in play girls learn that to be competitive is considered unfeminine (Lipman-
11
Blumen). It becomes apparent to girls that relationships are held at a high standard and are to be
considered more important than competition. As a result of learning that accomplishments are
met within the context of relationships, girls form appropriate life strategies to take on life’s
goals. In contrast, play for boys consists of a different set of guidelines in accordance to their
male gender role.
The nature of play in games for boys takes place in large groups with an emphasis on
competition (Richmond-Abbott, 1992). The characteristics of boys’ play include high structure
and high strategy while maintaining competition and cooperation. Boys will play with other
boys they may not like who can contribute skills to the group. This carries over into their future
ability to be a team player in the teenage years and on into adulthood. Again, boys are expected
to be tough and masculine while competing under any circumstances presented in order to win.
Girls and boys use the socialization lessons from their childhood to help guide them into their
adolescent years.
Throughout adolescence the messages boys learn in childhood continue to be important
in order for boys to be considered masculine (Richmond-Abbott, 1992). The characteristic of
toughness through physical appearance is sometimes necessary to prove oneself as masculine in
physical fights and/or organized sports. An adolescent boy who has a strong body-build gains
popularity because it is desirable to be an athletic boy. Boys who are physically attractive are
also more popular because more positive attributions are made about those who are physically
attractive. Another realm that is associated with social prestige in adolescence is sports. A boy
who is involved in sports continues to access the lessons learned in childhood play in order to
achieve the goal of winning.
An additional aspect of the adolescent boy which is important in Gender Role Theory is
the concept of “being cool” (Richmond-Abbott, 1992). Boys are taught that expressing emotions
is not appropriate for their gender. It is necessary to show that one can be calm in all situations
to prove ones fearlessness and adequacy. Being cool is another example that allows boys to
12
demonstrate their masculinity. Adolescent girls also continue to demonstrate the social
messages learned in childhood.
Physical attractiveness determines social prestige with girls just as with boys (Richmond-
Abbott, 1992). Body image is an important factor that causes adolescent girls to have low self-
esteem. A girl who considers herself to be unattractive and have a poor body generally also does
not consider herself worthy as a human being. Girls also place great importance upon “being
liked,” which is associated with popularity. A problem with these adolescent ideals that girls
think lead to femininity is low control regarding results. In order to achieve popularity, a girl
depends upon a boy’s recognition. Girls and boys use the socialization lessons from their
childhood and adolescence and translate the lessons into appropriate gender-typed roles as
adults.
There is a period in adolescence that is described as the “generation gap” between parents
and children (Lipman-Blumen, 1984). Lipman-Blumen suggests that a power struggle between
the parents’ gender role values versus the children’s generational values takes place. As children
grow, their value systems are influenced by both peers and parents. As children grow older, the
generation gap decreases in strength (Lipman-Blumen). As the children become adults, they
adopt a combination of interpersonal, family, and social values based on experiences from their
own generation. They also are influenced by gender role experiences that have taken place in
their life. Families are the place where children learn most about the gender roles they will take
on as adults.
Lipman-Blumen (1984) describes the socialization of gender roles as a lifelong
experience during which adults go through different phases. Although different combinations of
families exist more often today than they did 50 years ago, the socialization of specific gender
roles has still maintained the basic elements. First, one will learn the difference between being a
girl or a boy. Second, one will learn that one is a girl or boy. Third, one will learn the different
13
expectations based on being a girl or a boy. Gender Role Theory is able to provide support for
the effects of family violence and divorce on marital expectations.
Boys are socialized that aggression is an acceptable means to achieving goals. This
message continues into adolescence as boys are taught that sometimes physical aggression is a
way to prove themselves in relationships. Some may carry masculinity to extremes and resort to
using violence in intimate relationships. As a result of the socialization that has taken place over
the lifespan, males may consider violence in intimate relationships a way of continuing to
achieve goals at any cost.
On the other hand, girls are socialized to be caretakers. In addition, girls are also
socialized that it is the norm for boys to be aggressive (Lipman-Blumen, 1984). As a result, girls
often follow instead of lead and learn to become women that are also followers. Girls and
women are taught to be second-class citizens in social and sexual situations. For example, being
the quarterback’s girlfriend or the doctor’s wife may bring acknowledgment from the association
to a male and not a woman’s individual characteristics. Therefore, the pattern that takes place
perpetuates and forces women to rely on being caretakers while accepting aggressive behavior in
men.
One might expect that future marital expectations will be impacted as a result of the
socialization of boys and girls who have acquired idealistic, pessimistic, or realistic expectations
about their roles in future relationships. As caretakers, women may excuse or try to fix the
aggressive behavior of men. Men may continue to exhibit aggressive behavior because of the
socialization they have been taught. Families are the place where children learn most about the
gender roles they will adopt as adults (Lipman-Blumen, 1984). Just as children are socialized to
learn gender roles throughout stages of life, children learn future marital expectations from
different experiences witnessed in their family.
Girls who witness aggressive behavior from their fathers toward their mothers may use
this experience as a guide regarding gender roles and future marital expectations. In addition,
14
boys who witness aggressive behavior from their fathers toward their mothers may also be
influenced by this experience. Also, a child who experiences the divorce of his or her parents
may learn lessons that contribute to the development of pessimistic, idealistic, or realistic future
marital expectations. Hence, a child may use his or her own family of origin as a guide to direct
the development of future marital expectations in his or her family of origin. Therefore, the
attitudes and behaviors of Gender Role Theory may influence the impact that family violence
and divorce have on future marital expectations.
Social Learning Theory
Social Learning Theory contributes to the present research by reinforcing the influence of
social experiences in the process of acquiring pessimistic, idealistic, or realistic future marital
expectations. Bandura (1977) discussed the process of developing behavior through either
firsthand experience or attending to another’s behavior. Social learning by response
consequences takes place when individuals observe or experience behaviors causing either
positive or negative responses. Specific behaviors can be chosen or eliminated based on this
process of differential reinforcement. As a result, many roles are achieved through response
consequences such as passing on knowledge, provoking motivation via rewards, and
immediately causing responses to strengthen.
The role of passing on knowledge through response consequences enables individuals to
attend to various responses pertaining to specific behaviors. In addition, individuals are able to
develop decisions about behaviors in certain settings, which will steer future behavior. Next,
response consequences work to motivate individuals to perform certain behaviors based on
possible consequences, rewards, or lack of both, based on previous experiences. This role of
response consequences causes individuals to pay attention to situations that will result in the
highest reward. Last, an individual’s willingness to perform certain behaviors is strengthened
when one becomes mindful of rewards connected to the behavior.
15
Bandura (1977) stated, “Fortunately, most human behavior is learned observationally
through modeling: from observing others one forms an idea of how new behaviors are
performed, and on later occasions this coded information serves as a guide for action” (p. 22).
Bandura identified four operations of observational learning, including attention, retention,
motor reproduction, and motivation processes, which result in the acquisition of knowledge.
Attentional processes are important because the one observing must be aware of the
details of the behavior taking place (Elliott, Kratochwill, Cook, & Travers, 2000). One will be
able to imitate this behavior if close attention is paid to the details of the model. Second, an
observer needs to be able to form a picture in his or her mind of the behavior being observed.
This allows for the observer to retain the behavior so it can be imitated. Third, motor
reproduction processes allow an observer to reproduce the observed behavior. Elliott et al.
recognized that motor reproduction is important in order for “learning’s mechanisms to be used:
stimulus-cognition-response-reinforcement” (p. 222). Fourth, motivational processes consist of
an observer being motivated by the circumstances surrounding a specific behavior. As a result,
the observer will be motivated to perform a behavior based on the incentives he or she may
achieve whether through vicarious or self-reinforcement.
Unlike operant conditioning, reinforcement in observational learning can function as a
preceding effect. Observational learning concentrates on ones ability to perform desired
behaviors based on anticipation of rewards that have been previously identified. The first
element of reinforcement is a preceding incentive. Second, the preceding incentive is followed
by attending to an ideal stimulus. Third, the individual proceeds to complete symbolic coding,
mental organization, and drilling. Finally, a response in the form of a specific behavior results
from the process. Additional paths of reinforcement are present in observational learning
according to Social Leaning Theory such as external, vicarious, and self-reinforcement.
External reinforcement is an effective way to produce desired behaviors through the use
of incentives (Wolman, 1977). Various schedules of reinforcement result in varied behavior.
16
Types of incentives may include social acceptance, lack of privileges, praise, social rejection,
tangible reward, punishment, social affect, and social attention. Vicarious reinforcement causes
individuals to select and reject certain behaviors based on their observation of how successful or
negative it was for others. Individuals who observe another enact a behavior with negative
consequences may choose to change certain aspects of the behavior hoping they might obtain
success. Self-reinforcement allows individuals to set consequences for ones own behavior.
Reward, punishment, or lack of consequence is decided based on the importance of the behavior
performed by the individual.
One individual can cause another to develop anticipated expectations based on hearsay
regarding an experience (Bandura, 1977). For example, a person who has experienced suffering
from an event may influence another’s preconception about an event. Expectancy learning takes
place either symbolically or vicariously. Often individuals will behave specifically toward an
event based on preconceived symbolic ideas that lack firsthand experience. Vicarious
expectancy learning involves behavior resulting from directly observing another individual in a
specific situation. The one observing will adopt the behavior of the model and apply it to similar
situations.
A person’s environment, behavior, and personal factors equally influence one another.
Bandura (1977) termed this concept reciprocal determinism in order to describe the process one
uses to make decisions regarding which behaviors he or she is going to choose to perform.
Consequently, the goal is for individuals to make decisions that will result in the best possible
outcome regarding behavior.
Social Learning Theory contributes to the topics of family violence, divorce, and marital
expectations by reinforcing the influence of social experiences in the process of acquiring
pessimistic, idealistic, or realistic future marital expectations. Behaviors that a child may
experience firsthand or observe from another person contribute to the development of
expectations for his or her own behavior in similar situations. Based on whether the experienced
17
or observed events are positive or negative, children may adopt expectations that will continue to
guide their behavior. Response consequences may affect a child’s expectations through
information about experiences, rewards from certain behavior, or through the strengthening of
responses for certain behavior.
Children are reinforced by many role models, but their parents are the main
reinforcement in children’s lives (Richmond-Abbott, 1992). If violence is experienced or
observed, it may be replicated based on whether it was perceived to be associated with positive
or negative consequences. In a family where violence is a frequent occurrence, children may
choose to imitate this behavior based on their perception of its effectiveness as a conflict tactic.
Divorce is another event a child may experience that will influence his or her future
behaviors based on effects experienced directly or observed in other family members. Children
also may observe effects of divorce in peers. The reinforcement consequences connected to
various experiences associated with divorce are the key to children choosing to perform behavior
that may affect their own marital expectations. Expectations about relationships that children
experience or derive from their experiences with others will guide future action in their own
relationships. Positive and negative experiences may cause children to perform certain
behaviors that may cause idealistic, pessimistic, or realistic marital expectations to be formed.
Family Systems Theory
Family Systems Theory is a theory of human behavior that focuses on the family as an
emotional unit in order to explain the multifaceted interactions in this unit (Kerr, 2002). The
family is considered an emotional unit because of the powerful emotional connection its
members share, regardless of their feelings about being emotionally close with each other. The
powerful emotional connections experienced by each family influences the beliefs, feelings, and
behaviors of the family members in an intense way. The family members seek the attention,
approval, and support of each other. In addition, the family members react to each member’s
18
needs, expectations, and distress. As a result, the family members experience interdependence
based on connected and reactive interactions.
At times, families use their interdependence in order to achieve cooperation with one
another (Kerr, 2002). At other times, the interdependent nature of the family can cause problems
as a result of increased conflict. Certain family members may try to mediate the negative effects,
which may include feeling overwhelmed, lonely, or out of control. A particular member may
feel compelled to absorb the anxiety caused by the family conflict. Consequently, this family
member may develop psychiatric, medical, or social neuroses (Kerr, 2003).
A family’s level of interdependence produces individual family members who have
different levels of differentiation of self (Kerr, 2003). Differentiation of self can be described as
the ability of a person to achieve independent functioning without the ongoing need for
emotional reinforcement from others (Kerr & Bowen, 1988). A person with a strong
differentiated self is able to resist relationship pressures and think about situations objectively.
This concept is important to illustrate the family’s influence over how its members think, feel,
and behave, which impacts future thoughts, feelings, and behavior (Kerr, 2002).
Another concept in Family Systems Theory is the nuclear family emotional system. The
nuclear family emotional system explains specific relationship patterns of behavior within a
family that cause problems to develop (Kerr, 2002). The four patterns addressed in the nuclear
family emotional system are marital conflict, dysfunction in one spouse, impairment of one or
more children, and emotional distance. The pattern of emotional functioning of a family will
determine which pattern(s) of behavior will develop as a problem within a family (Kerr &
Bowen, 1988). As conflict increases within a family, problems in the four relationship patterns
will increase (Kerr, 2003). Thus, the four relationship patterns cause anxiety in the family to be
placed in specific areas of the family (Kerr, 2002). This concept is important because it provides
support for the role families play in causing individuals to develop dysfunctional relationship
patterns in their family of creation based on their family-of-origin experiences.
19
The final concept in Family Systems Theory that contributes to the present research is
called multigenerational transmission process. Multigenerational transmission process explains
the connection between individual differences and multigenerational trends in functioning,
which are exhibited by family members throughout different generations (Kerr & Bowen, 1988).
The multigenerational transmission process takes place on various levels, such as deliberate
teaching and learning as well as habitual and unconscious programming of emotional reactions
and behaviors (Kerr, 2002). Deliberate and unconscious programming in a family results in
children absorbing and responding to the attitudes, behaviors, and values of their parents.
Therefore, siblings develop various levels of differentiation of self. Usually, one sibling has a
higher level and one sibling has a lower level of differentiation of self (Kerr & Bowen).
Subsequently, individuals choose mates with a similar level of differentiation of self
(Kerr, 2002). This process of selection results in different siblings having marriages that have
different levels of differentiation. As the relationship patterns that caused the siblings and their
marriages to achieve certain levels of differentiation continue, their children will achieve
different levels of differentiation (Kerr & Bowen, 1988). This process will continue to take
place throughout future generations resulting in more stable families for people who are highly
self-differentiated (Kerr, 2002). Individuals who have a higher level of self differentiation are
more productive members of society. On the other hand, individuals who have a lower level of
self differentiation are more unstable and dependent on others and experience more problems in
life.
The level of differentiation that a person achieves can affect his or her overall functioning
and success in every aspect of life (Kerr, 2002). Therefore, multigenerational transmission
process is important to explain how the family unit may transmit future marital expectations.
Specifically, the experience of a divorce may affect the level of differentiation a person achieves.
As a result, that person may develop dysfunctional relationship patterns that cause pessimistic or
idealistic future marital expectations. The same process may take place because a person
20
witnessed family violence in his or her family of origin. Consequently, a person may develop
distorted future marital expectations that do not reflect realistic attitudes and beliefs about
marriage. The consequences of this progression may entail increased marital instability and
family violence, which may be transmitted to future generations through the multigenerational
transmission process.
Empirical Findings
Importance of Conflict
That there is a generally negative impact on children who have witnessed a physically
violent act between their parents has been strongly supported through research. Henning,
Leitenberg, Coffey, Turner, and Bennett (1996) surveyed 617 women in regard to witnessing
parental violence prior to 16 years of age. Out of the 617 women, 123 reported witnessing a
physically violent act between their parents. The participants were also given measures that
assessed their psychological and social adjustment. The results indicated that the women who
reported witnessing a physically violent act between their parents presented with lower
psychological and social adjustment than women who reported never observing violence
between their parents. The researchers obtained these results after controlling for other family
issues that may have caused psychological and social maladjustment such as child abuse,
viewing parental verbal arguments, and the child’s perception of the level of parental caring. A
limitation of this study was the absence of controlling for other possible family issues that could
contribute to long-term psychological or social maladjustment, such as alcohol and drug abuse,
parental mental health problems, and parental legal involvement.
McCloskey, Figueredo, and Koss (1995) studied mothers and their children in battered
women’s shelters. The researchers were interested in exploring the different problems that
children experience and the ability for family support to help the children weather the problems.
Results support a connection between the mother’s and children’s mental health problems as
21
being related to the family’s experienced or witnessed violence. The children’s mental health
was assessed using the Child Behavior Checklist and the Child Assessment Schedule. The
mothers’ reported mental health issues were assessed using the Brief Symptom Inventory.
Scores on these inventories reflected general psychological symptoms for the mothers and
children. McCloskey et al. (1995) conceptualized children’s mental problems as varying
between numerous internalizing and externalizing processes. Internalizing processes can be
defined as turning an issue toward oneself and dealing with it in a way, such as through
depression. Externalizing issues can be defined as when an issue is dealt with by turning
outward to others, such as in the case of interpersonal aggression or angry outbursts. It was also
found that when children exposed to family violence were given family support it did not ease
the psychological effects of the violence. Importantly, McCloskey et al. pointed out that this
exposure to family violence may psychosocially delay children in a way that may affect
adjustment in adulthood.
Researchers have also looked at the relationship among witnessing parental conflict,
child coping, and child adjustment. O’Brien, Margolin, and John (1995) investigated these
variables with 83 families where the children reported on frequency and intensity of witnessed
conflict, coping skills, and symptoms related to mental health problems. The parents reported on
child hostility, internalizing, and externalizing behaviors. The results indicated that children
who used strategies that caused them to be included in parental conflict had higher levels of
depression, anxiety, hostility, and low self-esteem. The parents also indicated higher levels of
child hostility. On the other hand, those children who used coping skills that caused them to be
independent of parental conflict had decreased levels of anxiety. When a child turned to sources
outside the family for support, the mother indicated fewer internalizing behaviors. This study
provides support for the idea that maladjustment can result when children are drawn into the
middle of parental conflict.
22
Divorce
The effects children experience as a result of divorce can range from increased coping
skills on one end to psychosocial problems on the other end. Schwartz and Kaslow (1997) point
out that the effects of divorce on children do not always have to be negative. Some children
make the transition from an intact to non-intact home with increased coping skills. On the other
hand, even though divorce may be the best solution for a dissolved marriage, it often leaves
long-lasting effects on children that may take place in the form of psychosocial problems. One
of the possible psychosocial problems may include idealistic or pessimistic future marital
expectations that negatively affect relationships and future marriages.
It is possible that gender differences may impact ones attitude concerning divorce. Black
and Sprenkle (1991) studied the attitudes of college students from intact and non-intact homes.
Students from both intact and non-intact homes viewed divorce in a neutral or indifferent
manner. Men were more favorable in their attitudes concerning divorce than women.
Experiencing parental divorce at an earlier age resulted in a more neutral attitude toward divorce.
The non-intact group experienced more intergenerational marital instability as evidenced by data
from grandparent and parental marital status. The study is important to point out the gender
differences in attitudes toward divorce.
Marital Expectations
Kozuch and Cooney (1995) reinforce the idea that ones marital attitudes are altered
depending on parental marital status and the level of famiy conflict in the home. Kozuch and
Cooney found that children from divorced families who reported high conflict were more likely
to believe it is acceptable to cohabitate before marriage than the participants whose parents were
not divorced. The participants from divorced homes also believed it was less important to be
brought up by two parents than the participants from intact homes. Individuals from intact
homes who reported a high level of conflict agreed less on a statement that marriage was a
23
lifetime commitment than individuals from non-intact homes who reported a low level of
conflict. Participants from the divorced group did not believe marriage had to be a lifetime
commitment and reported more acceptance of the possibility of permanent singlehood than
participants from intact homes. Individuals who reported low family conflict were less accepting
of divorce than participants who reported high family conflict. These results highlight some
specific ways in which marital beliefs may be molded by parental divorce and/or family conflict.
Livingston and Kordinak (1990) researched marital role expectations to assess the long-
term effects of divorce on young adults as a way to explain the intergenerational transmission of
marital instability. The results showed a three-way interaction among gender, level of
religiosity, and parental divorce status. It was found that males from non-intact homes who were
more religious had more egalitarian marital role expectations. Livingston and Kordinak suggest
that the role-model rationale be expanded to increase support for the connection between the
role-model rationale and intergenerational transmission of marital instability.
Jones and Nelson (1996) studied college students’ marital expectations from intact and
non-intact homes. All participants completed the Love Attitudes Scale, Dean Romanticism
Scale, and the Marriage Expectation Scale. Jones and Nelson were not able to support their
hypothesis that college students from non-intact homes would hold more pessimistic or idealistic
expectations of marriage. In addition, support was not provided for students from intact homes
to have more realistic expectations of marriage. A lack of family conflict as an independent
variable was a limitation of this study that may have contributed to the results. Jones and Nelson
were able to establish the Marriage Expectation Scale as a valid research tool for assessing future
marital expectations during this study. The validity of the scale was based on the equally
internal consistent performance of the Marriage Expectation Scale, Love Attitudes Scale, and
Dean Romanticism Scale in the study.
24
Statement of Problem
Family violence and divorce are both problems in American society. In 2001, the
National Center for Health Statistics reported that divorces took place at more than half the rate
of marriages. Since 1950, the divorce rate has increased from 2.6 to 4.0 per 1,000 of the total
population. This increase reflects the growing problem of divorce in the last 50 years for the
American family. It is not just the event of divorce that causes negative consequences for the
children involved.
The presence of family conflict and/or violence also is a growing problem that concerns
many children. Data from the National Family Violence surveys in 1975 and 1985 reflect that
each year over 10 million children in America witness a physically violent act between their
parents (Straus, 1992). As a result of being exposed to family violence and/or divorce, children
may have difficulty forming realistic expectations about interpersonal relationships, which may
cause them to develop pessimistic or idealistic future marital expectations. Based on the
information concerning family violence and divorce as problems in American society, this study
will look further into the issues of reported historical family-of-origin violence and divorce in
terms of their effects on future marital expectations.
Hypotheses
Design I
1. Men and women will differ on marital expectations as measured by mean scores on
the Marriage Expectations Scale.
2. Participants from non-intact homes will report more idealistic marital expectations
than participants from intact homes.
3. Participants who report violence in their family of origin will report more
pessimistic marital expectations than participants who do not report violence.
25
4. Participants who report physical abuse in their family of origin will report more
pessimistic marital expectations than participants who do not report abuse.
Design II
5. Males and females will differ on frequency of assignment to idealistic, realistic, and
pessimistic marital expectations.
6. More participants who reported family violence will be classified as having
pessimistic marital expectations on the Marriage Expectations Scale than
participants who did not report family violence.
7. More participants who reported physical abuse will be classified as having
pessimistic marital expectations on the Marriage Expectations Scale than
participants who did not report physical abuse.
8. More participants from non-intact homes will be classified as having idealistic
marital expectations on the Marriage Expectations Scale than participants from
intact homes.
26
CHAPTER 2
METHODS
Participants
The participants were 293 students (male = 110, female = 183) enrolled in introductory
psychology courses at a southeastern university. The mean age of the participants was 19.67
with a range of 18 years of age to 45 years of age. The mean age was very skewed in the 18-19-
year-old category with 196 participants falling in this age range. In the study, 89.4% of the
participants were Caucasian, 4.8% African American, 3.1% Other, 1.4% Asian American, and
1.4% Hispanic American. There were 26 participants excluded for data analysis because they
had previously been married.
Measures
The Marriage Expectation Scale (MES), which measures future marital expectations, was
used in this study (Jones & Nelson, 1996). Jones and Nelson studied college students’ marriage
expectations from intact and non-intact homes with the purpose of establishing the Marriage
Expectation Scale (MES). The scale is designed to assess the marriage expectations of
individuals who have never been married. The scale consists of 40 items rated on a 5-point
Likert scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). The items assess
expectations of various elements of marriage such as compatibility, equality, and intimacy. Each
item was profiled to fall into one of the three categories by a panel of experts in the field. All
items were recoded from a 5-point scale to a 3-point scale. Pessimistic responses were coded as
one; realistic responses were coded as two; and idealistic responses were coded as three. A
participant’s score could range from 40-120. Mean scores in the highest range (80-120) indicate
unrealistic-idealistic marriage expectations. Mean scores from the middle (40-80) indicate
realistic marriage expectations. Mean scores from the lowest range (<40) indicate unrealistic-
27
pessimistic marriage expectations. The alpha coefficient for the MES was +.80 during the pilot
test and +.79 during the study. The data support a high internal consistency for this scale.
A second measure used in this study was the Conflict Tactics Scales (Straus, 1979). This
scale measures the self-reported presence of violence in the participant’s family of origin. The
participant responded to statements inquiring about the number of times the participant’s parents
used various conflict tactics at any point while the participants were living at home. The
responses were based on parent-to-parent interaction. The conflict tactics statements are broken
down into three categories including discussion, verbal aggression, and violence.
The presence of violence in a participant’s family of origin is established when it is
reported that any conflict tactics in the “violence subscale” have been used. The definition of
violence was looked at from two levels using the Conflict Tactics Scales. First, violence was
defined using a broader definition that included violent physical acts likely to cause pain (throw
something at him/her, slapping). Each participant only had to report one occurrence of a violent
physical act to fall under the broader definition of family violence. Second, violence was more
narrowly defined as physical abuse. This definition was a more severe definition that included
acts likely to cause injury, such as kicking, hitting with a fist, using a knife, and biting. Again,
each participant only had to report one occurrence of a physically abusive act to fall into the
more narrowly defined physical abuse group.
Each participant recorded the frequency of each conflict tactic based on a scale ranging
from 0 (never) to 6 (more than 20 times). For the items marked 0 (never), the participant
recorded if the conflict tactic had ever happened. The alpha coefficients reported for form N for
the discussion category were reported to fall within .52-.82. The verbal aggression category was
reported to fall within .44-.85. The violent category was reported to fall within .79-.91. The
data support a high internal consistency for this scale.
A demographic questionnaire was used to assess the marital status of the participant’s
parents. If the participant’s parents were divorced, the participant reported his or her age at the
28
time of the divorce. Only participants over the age of 6 at the time of their parents’ divorce were
included in the study due to the operational definition for grouping non-intact participants. In
addition, the participant’s marital status was assessed. Due to the design and purpose of the
Marriage Expectation Scale, which measures future marital expectations, only the participants
who reported having never been married were included for data analysis. Also, the participant’s
gender, age, race, and level of education were assessed.
Procedure
Prior to collecting data, Institutional Review Board approval was obtained. The
demographic questionnaire, Marriage Expectation Scale, and Conflict Tactics Scale made up the
packet of materials given to the participants. The participants were offered extra credit in their
introductory psychology course for their involvement in the study per the policies of the
Department of Psychology. Alternative arrangements for commensurate extra credit were made
between the student and professor if the student did not wish to participate. The participants
were instructed by the experimenter to complete the packet of materials and respond honestly
and accurately. The participants were instructed to turn in their packet of materials at the front
of the classroom when finished. A handout was distributed along with the packet of materials
explaining that the participants did not have to answer questions that made them feel
uncomfortable. Contact information was also given for the purpose of answering any questions
or needs concerning the study or to learn about the results of the study if desired.
Research Design
Design I
A 2 x 2 x 2 x 2 Univariate Analysis of Variance (ANOVA) was conducted to compare
groups for mean score differences in marital expectations. Independent variables were male
versus female, intact versus non-intact home of origin, violent versus non-violent, and abuse
versus non-abuse.
29
Design II
Because the Marriage Expectation Scale is probably easiest to understand as a nominal
variable, a Chi-square design was used to determine the impact of independent variables on
frequency distributions in idealistic, realistic, and pessimistic marriage expectation categories.
Independent variables were gender, violence/abuse, intact/non-intact.
30
CHAPTER 3
RESULTS
In research design I, a 2 x 2 x 2 x 2 Univariate Analysis of Variance (ANOVA) was
conducted to compare groups for mean score differences in marital expectations. Independent
variables were male versus female, intact versus non-intact home of origin, violent versus non-
violent, and abuse versus non-abuse. Hypothesis 1 stated that men and women would differ on
marital expectations as measured by mean scores on the Marriage Expectations Scale.
Hypothesis 1 was not supported, F(1, 279) = .15, p >.05 (see Table 1). Hypothesis 2 stated that
participants from non-intact homes would report more idealistic marital expectations than
participants from intact homes. Hypothesis 2 was not supported, F(1, 279) = .68, p >.05 (see
Table 1). Hypothesis 3 stated that participants who reported violence in their family of origin
would report more pessimistic marital expectations than participants who did not report violence.
Hypothesis 3 was not supported, F(1, 279) = .31, p >.05 (see Table 1). Hypothesis 4 stated that
participants who reported physical abuse in their family of origin would report more pessimistic
marital expectations than participants who did not report abuse. Hypothesis 4 was not supported,
F(1, 279) = .12, p >.05 (see Table 1).
In research design II, because the Marriage Expectation Scale is probably easiest to
understand as a nominal variable, a Chi-square design was used to determine the impact of
independent variables on frequency distributions in idealistic, realistic, and pessimistic marriage
expectation categories. Independent variables were gender, violence/abuse, intact/non-intact.
Hypothesis 5 stated that males and females will differ on frequency of assignment to idealistic,
realistic, and pessimistic marital expectations. Hypothesis 5 was supported x
2
(279) = 9.95, p
>.007. The main difference was found in participants who reported realistic marital
expectations, with 121 females and 53 males assigned to this category. In contrast, there were
59 females and 52 males assigned to the pessimistic marital expectations category. Further, few
31
participants reported idealistic marital expectations with 3 females and 5 males being assigned to
this category. Hypothesis 6 stated that more participants who reported family violence would be
classified as having pessimistic marital expectations than participants who did not report family
violence. Hypothesis 6 was not supported, x
2
(279) = 4.83, p >.05. Hypothesis 7 stated that more
participants who reported physical abuse would be classified as having pessimistic marital
expectations than participants who did not report physical abuse. Hypothesis 7 was not
supported, x
2
(279) = 5.61, p >.05. Hypothesis 8 stated that more participants from non-intact
homes would be classified as having idealistic marital expectations than participants from intact
homes. Hypothesis 8 was not supported, x
2
(279) = .08, p >.05.
32
Table 1
Analysis of Variance for Design I
Type III
Sum of squares
df
Mean square F Sig.
Sex
Hypothesis
5.054
1
5.054
.15
NS
Parents
Hypothesis
23.685
1
23.685
.68
NS
Violent
Hypothesis
10.68
1
10.968
.31
NS
Abusive
Hypothesis
4.242
1
4.242
.12
NS
Sex * Parents
Hypothesis
133.265
1
133.265
3.82
NS
Sex * Violent
Hypothesis
95.989
1
95.989
2.75
NS
Sex * Abusive
Hypothesis
35.685
1
35.685
1.02
NS
Parents * Violent
Hypothesis
4.096
1
4.096
.12
NS
Parents * Abusive
Hypothesis
29.557
1
29.557
.85
NS
Sex * Parents *
Violent
Hypothesis
1.182
1
1.182
.03
NS
Sex * Parents *
Abusive
Hypothesis
119.440
1
119.440
3.42
NS
33
Table 2
Cell Numbers for Analysis of Variance for Design I
Value label
N
1 = female 1.00
1 = male 2.00
parents
married 1.00
parents
divorced 2.00
violent .00
1.00
abusive .00
1.00
No violence
Yes violence
No abuse
Abusive
183
110
207
86
150
143
236
57
Table 3
Mean Scores for Marriage Expectation Scale: Analysis of Variance
1 = female, 2 = male
Violent
Mean
1.00
87.503
no violence
yes violence
87.718
87.288
2.00
86.881
no violence
yes violence
88.149
85.613
34
CHAPTER 4
DISCUSSION
The expected outcomes based on the hypotheses developed for this study were partially
fulfilled. First, hypotheses 1 and 5, which were related to the impact of gender on marital
expectations, were partially fulfilled. In design II, Chi-square results indicated that a significant
difference existed between men and women on marital expectations. Therefore, hypothesis 5
was confirmed. The main difference in marital expectations among participants existed in the
realistic marital expectations category, with 121 females and 53 males being assigned to this
category. Therefore, the females were more realistic in their marital expectations. In addition,
the females were also more pessimistic in their marital expectations, with 59 females and 52
males being assigned to this category. Although few participants were assigned to the idealistic
marital expectations category, the males were more idealistic in their marital expectations, with 3
females and 5 males being assigned to this category. These results indicate that men and women
have very different ideas about marriage that need to be examined through further research.
However, when the same hypothesis was analyzed using an ANOVA, significant results were
not found.
Second, hypotheses 2 and 8, which were related to the impact of divorce on marital
expectations, were not fulfilled. These hypotheses were based on research by Jones and Nelson
(1996) that looked at marital expectations of college students from non-intact and intact homes.
The expected outcomes that participants from non-intact homes would report more idealistic
marital expectations than participants from intact homes was not fulfilled. Third, hypotheses 3,
4, 6, and 7, which were related to the impact of family violence and physical abuse on marital
expectations, were not fulfilled. Jones and Nelson, who looked at marital expectations of college
students from non-intact and intact homes, listed family conflict as a limitation of their study.
Therefore, the current research included family conflict as an independent variable. The
35
expected outcome that participants who reported family violence or physical abuse in their
family of origin would report more pessimistic marital expectations than participants who did
not report family violence or physical abuse was not fulfilled. However, an interaction was
found among gender, intact or non-intact family of origin, and physical abuse. This finding
indicates that these variables do appear to affect ideas about future marital expectations, which
need to be assessed further in future research.
Several limitations have had an influence on the findings in this research study. First, the
Marriage Expectations Scale (MES) appeared to be a weak instrument for several reasons. The
MES seemed to have a weak face validity, which may have steered the participants to answer the
items in a realistic manner even though they may have held other beliefs about marriage. As a
result, most of the participants reported realistic marital expectations on the MES. In addition,
the items on the MES do not appear to focus on elements that would make marriage work but on
elements that may be involved in finding a partner with many similarities. Further, the MES
placed a small amount of emphasis on factors outside marriage that may affect marital
expectations, such as other interpersonal relationships. Consequently, the MES should be
examined in further detail before being used in future research on marital expectations.
Another limitation of the present study included the process by which data were collected
regarding family violence. The researchers were not able to accurately assess when the reported
family violence or physical abuse took place because the participants were only asked to report
their parents’ marital status. As a result, many participants who reported family violence or
physical abuse may have witnessed these actions after their parents were divorced, among a
parent and a stepparent, or among their parents while they were married. This limitation did not
allow for an accurate assessment of the extent which the participants witnessed family violence
and physical abuse.
A final limitation included the sample of participants who were surveyed for this study.
The participants were selected from a college campus and did not reflect the general population.
36
In addition, the participants were all students in introductory psychology classes. As a result, the
participants also may not represent the general population of the university campus. Further, the
age of the participants was very skewed in the 18-19 category, with 196 participants falling in
this age range. Consequently, a limited age may have also affected the results in this study. A
more diverse age range may have allowed for more participants who have reflected on their life
experience and how it has affected their future marital expectations to be included.
Future research needs to be conducted in the area of marital expectations in order to gain
more information about the variables that impact an individual’s future marital expectations.
One suggestion for future research includes examining the differences among men and women
regarding marital expectations. Researchers need to obtain an accurate assessment of men’s and
women’s thoughts about marriage before other variables related to marriage are assessed. As a
result, a more reliable picture of variables that may affect marital expectations may arise.
Second, a solid instrument that takes into account all elements of marriage needs to be
used so that researchers are able to gain an accurate picture of the future marital expectations of
the participants used in marital expectation research. Consequently, a more extensive
assessment tool on marital expectations than the one used in this study needs to be developed. A
final suggestion for future research includes using a sample that represents the general
population and consists of a wide variety of age ranges. This suggestion will allow for the use of
a group of participants who may have more mature ideas about their future marital expectations
to be included.
37
REFERENCES
Bandura, A. (1977). Social learning theory. NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Black, L. E., & Sprenkle, D. H. (1991). Gender differences in college students’ attitudes toward
divorce and their willingness to marry. Journal of Divorce and Remarriage, 14(3-4), 47-
60.
DivorceMagazine.Com. (2000). U.S. divorce statistics. Retrieved July 20, 2004, from
http://www.divorcemag.com/statistics/statsUS.shtml
Elliott, S. N., Kratochwill, T. R., Cook, J. L., & Travers, J. F. (2000). Educational psychology:
Effective teaching, effective learning. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Fagan, P. F., & Rector, R. E. (2000). The effects of divorce on America. Retrieved September
3, 2004, from http://www.heritage.org/Research/Family/BG1373.cfm
Grych, J. H., & Fincham, F. D. (1990). Marital conflict and children’s adjustment: A
cognitive-contextual framework. Psychological Bulletin, 108, 267-290.
Henning, K., Leitenberg, H., Coffey, P., Turner, T., & Bennett, R. T. (1996). Long-term
psychological and social impact of witnessing physical conflict between parents. Journal
of Interpersonal Violence, 11(1), 35-51.
Jones, G. D., & Nelson, E. S. (1996). Expectations of marriage among college students from
intact and non-intact homes. Journal of Divorce and Remarriage, 26, 171-189.
Kerr, M. E. (2002). One family’s story: A primer on Bowen theory. Washington, DC:
Georgetown Family Center.
Kerr, M. E. (2003). Multigenerational family systems theory of Bowen and its application. In
G. P. Sholenar (Ed.), Textbook of family and couples therapy: Clinical applications (pp.
103-126). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Publishing.
Kerr, M. E., & Bowen, J. (1988). Family evaluation: An approach based on Bowen theory.
New York: WW Norton.
Kozuch, P., & Cooney, T. M. (1995). Young adults’ marital and family attitudes: The role of
recent parental divorce, and family and parental conflict. Journal of Divorce and
Remarriage, 23, 45-73.
Larson, J. H. (1988). The marriage quiz: College students’ beliefs in selected myths about
marriage. Family Relations, 37, 3-11.
Lipman-Blumen, J. (1984). Gender roles and power. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Livingston, R. B., & Kordinak, S. T. (1990). The long term effect of parental divorce: Marital
role expectations. Journal of Divorce and Remarriage, 14(2), 91-105.
Markland, S. R., & Nelson, E. S. (1993). The relationship between familial conflict and the
identity of young adults. Journal of Divorce and Remarriage, 20, 193-209.
38
McCloskey, L. A., Figueredo, A. J., & Koss, M. P. (1995). The effects of systemic family
violence on children’s mental health. Child Development, 66, 1239-1261.
O’Brien, M., Margolin, G., & John, R. S. (1995). Relation among marital conflict, child coping,
and child adjustment. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 24, 346-361.
Richmond-Abbott, M. (1992). Gender roles over the life cycle. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Schwartz, L. L., & Kaslow, F. W. (1997). Painful partings. New York: John Wiley & Sons.
Straus, M. A. (1979). Measuring intrafamily conflict and violence: The conflicts tactics (CT)
scales. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 41, 75-88.
Straus, J. A. (1992). Children as witnesses to marital violence: A risk factor for lifelong
problems among a nationally representative sample of American men and women. In
Schwartz, D. F. (Ed.), Children and violence. (pp. 98-109). Columbus, OH: Ross
Laboratories.
Wallerstein, J. S. (1987). Children of divorce: Report of a ten year follow-up of early latency-
age children. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 57, 199-211.
Wolman, B. (Ed). (1977). International encyclopedia of psychiatry, psychology,
psychoanalysis, and neurology. (Vols. 1-12). New York: Aesculapius Publishers.
39
APPENDICES
APPENDIX A
Demographic Questionnaire
Age: _____
Circle One:
Sex:
1) Female
2) Male
Circle One:
1) Freshman
2) Sophomore
3) Junior
4) Senior
5) Graduate
6) Other: ____________
Circle One:
1) African American
2) Asian
3) Hispanic
4) Caucasian
5) Other: ____________
Circle One:
Parents’ Marital Status:
1) Married
2) Divorced
3) Never married
4) Other: ____________
Circle One:
Your Marital Status:
1) Married
2) Single, Never Married
3) Single, Married Previously
4) Other: ____________
40
APPENDIX B
Conflict Tactics Scales (CTS)
No matter how well a family gets along, there are times when they disagree, get annoyed with each other, want
different things, or just have spats or fights because they are in a bad mood, are tired, or for some other reason.
Families also have many different ways of trying to settle their differences. This is a list of things that might
happen when having differences. Complete this questionnaire by thinking back to when you were growing up;
respond to the following items based on how you remember your family. If in doubt, rate your family as you
remember it as being in the last year you lived at home. Please circle how many times your parents did each
of these things in interaction with each other.
Parents during your childhood
1 = Once
2 = Twice
3 = 3-5 Times
4 = 6-10 Times
5 = 11-20 Times
6 = More than 20
0 = Never
A. Discussed an issue calmly 1 2 3 4 5 6 0
B. Got information to back up his/her side of things 1 2 3 4 5 6 0
C. Brought in, or tried to bring in, someone to help settle things 1 2 3 4 5 6 0
D. Insulted or swore at him/her 1 2 3 4 5 6 0
E. Sulked or refused to talk about an issue 1 2 3 4 5 6 0
F. Stomped out of the room or house or yard 1 2 3 4 5 6 0
G. Cried 1 2 3 4 5 6 0
H. Did or said something to spite him/her 1 2 3 4 5 6 0
I. Threatened to hit or throw something at him/her 1 2 3 4 5 6 0
J. Threw or smashed or hit or kicked something 1 2 3 4 5 6 0
K. Threw something at him/her 1 2 3 4 5 6 0
L. Pushed, grabbed or shoved him/her 1 2 3 4 5 6 0
M. Slapped him/her 1 2 3 4 5 6 0
N. Kicked, bit, or hit him/her with a fist 1 2 3 4 5 6 0
O. Hit or tried to hit him/her with something 1 2 3 4 5 6 0
P. Beat him/her 1 2 3 4 5 6 0
Q. Choked him/her 1 2 3 4 5 6 0
R. Threatened him/her with a knife or gun 1 2 3 4 5 6 0
S. Used a knife or fired a gun 1 2 3 4 5 6 0
41
APPENDIX C
Marriage Expectation Scale (MES)
Strongly Disagree Disagree Neutral Agree Strongly Agree
_______________
_____________________________________________
1 2 3 4 5
Please respond to the following statements using the above scale. Simply mark the response that first comes to your
mind. There are no “right” or “wrong” answers, and the statements may be interpreted differently according to
the individual. Please mark your answers on the computer-scored sheet using a No. 2 pencil. Imagining what
your future marriage might be like, mark the response that first comes to mind. Thank you!
_____ 1. My marriage will be more intense than any of my other close relationships.
_____ 2. We will both place the same amount of emphasis on sex.
_____ 3. My partner and I will be similar in our habits of cleanliness.
_____ 4. Keeping the finances straight will be difficult.
_____ 5. Asking each other for help will not be a problem.
_____ 6. My partner will be quite attractive.
_____ 7. We will have certain household chores that each of us will do.
_____ 8. Time alone will not be as important as time together.
_____ 9. Maintaining romantic love will be a key factor to our marital happiness.
_____ 10. My spouse will want to have children at the same time I do.
_____ 11. My partner will absolutely be willing to “follow me” to another city if I’m promoted.
_____ 12. Our marital satisfaction will be reflected by our sex life.
_____ 13. My partner will have a great sense of humor.
_____ 14. We will both be willing to see a marriage counselor if necessary.
_____ 15. My spouse and I will be quite affectionate with each other.
_____ 16. Having children will improve marital satisfaction for both of us.
_____ 17. My spouse will instinctively know what I want and need to be happy.
_____ 18. My partner will have trouble understanding me.
42
_____ 19. It will not bother me if my spouse loses his or her “shape.”
_____ 20. My partner will cherish me.
_____ 21. My partner will always listen to me.
_____ 22. I will be able to change my partner by pointing out his/her shortcomings.
_____ 23. We will get angry with each other.
_____ 24. Sex will always be exciting.
_____ 25. We will always express feelings openly.
_____ 26. We will always agree about whose side of the family we will spend holidays with.
_____ 27. Decisions will be made together at all times.
_____ 28. I will be suspicious of my partner’s fidelity.
_____ 29. All our fights will be resolved quickly.
_____ 30. My partner will forget important dates such as our anniversary.
_____ 31. My spouse will automatically like my side of the family.
_____ 32. We will share equally the household chores.
_____ 33. My spouse will always consult me when making decisions.
_____ 34. We will always have extreme emotional closeness.
_____ 35. My spouse and I will argue a lot.
_____ 36. My partner and I will eat meals together all the time.
_____ 37. We will share all of the same interests.
_____ 38. I will have trouble getting along with the in-laws.
_____ 39. My partner will agree with me if I tell him or her to change something about him/herself.
_____ 40. My spouse will never be attracted to people of the opposite sex.
43
VITA
HOLLIE N. DILLON
Personal Data: Date of Birth: November 27, 1979
Place of Birth: Bristol, Virginia
Marital Status: Single
Education: Virginia High School, Bristol, Virginia
King College, Bristol, Tennessee; Psychology, B.A., 2001
East Tennessee State University, Johnson City, Tennessee;
Clinical Psychology, M.A., 2005
Professional
Experience: Psychiatric Technician, Woodridge Hospital, Johnson City, Tennessee, 2000 -
2001
Graduate Assistant, Center for Adult Programs and Services, East Tennessee
State University, Johnson City, Tennessee, 2001 – 2002
In-Home Counselor, Youth Villages, Johnson City, Tennessee, 2002 – 2003
Resident Director, Educational Talent Search Summer Camp, East Tennessee
State University, Johnson City, Tennessee, 2002 – 2004
PK-16 Council Research Assistant, East Tennessee State University, Johnson
City, Tennessee, 2003
Graduate Assistant Advisor, College of Arts and Sciences, East Tennessee State
University, Johnson City, Tennessee, 2003 - 2005
Presentations: Smoking and Suicidal Ideation Among College Students, Southeastern
Psychological Association; New Orleans, Louisiana, 2003
Honors and
Awards: Who’s Who Among America’s Colleges and Universities
Psi Chi Honor Society, King College, President
Kappa Delta Pi