48
Early childhood transitions
research: A review of concepts,
theory, and practice
By Pia Vogler, Gina Crivello and Martin Woodhead
WORKING PAPERS IN
Early Childhood Development
Cover: Little girl from the El Coro district in San Salvador, El Salvador. Photo: Jon Spaull
Design: Valetti, vormgeving en communicatie, The Hague, The Netherlands
Editing and proofreading: Green Ink (www.greenink.co.uk)
48
Early childhood transitions
research: A review of concepts,
theory, and practice
By Pia Vogler, Gina Crivello and
Martin Woodhead
May 2008
WORKING PAPERS IN
Early Childhood Development
Copyright © 2008 by the Bernard van Leer Foundation, The Netherlands. The Bernard van Leer Foundation encourages
fair use of this material. Proper citation is requested. This publication may not be resold for profit.
About the authors
Pia Vogler is a research assistant for ‘Young Lives’ and a DPhil candidate in the Department of International Development
(QEH), University of Oxford. She has been a consultant for UNHCR and is currently carrying out doctoral research on the
daily, seasonal and life course transitions of Karen children in Thailand.
Gina Crivello is an anthropologist and Child Research Coordinator for ‘Young Lives’, based at the Department of
International Development
(QEH), University of Oxford. Her research interests include the gender and inter-generational
dynamics of child migration and youth transitions in developing-country contexts.
Martin Woodhead is Professor of Childhood Studies at The Open University and Child Research Director for ‘Young Lives’.
His research and publications include early childhood development, education and care, children’s rights and child labour.
He is co-editor of the journal Children & Society, a member of the editorial board for Childhood and advisory board for
Journal of Early Childhood Research.
The authors are members of the research team for ‘Young Lives’, a 15-year longitudinal study of child poverty in Ethiopia,
India (Andhra Pradesh), Peru and Vietnam, including quantitative and qualitative studies related to service access,
transitions and well-being (see p.
24).
Citation
Vogler, P., Crivello, G. and Woodhead, M. (2008) Early childhood transitions research: A review of concepts, theory, and
practice. Working Paper No.
48. The Hague, The Netherlands: Bernard van Leer Foundation.
ISSN 1383-7907
ISBN 978-90-6195-103-2
Contents
Executive summary
Introduction
Chapter
1: Development and transition
Chapter
2: The structure of transitions
Chapter
3: Transitions within systems
Chapter
4: Childrens participation in transitions
Conclusion
Glossary: Some major concepts relevant to transitions research
Bibliography
v
1
5
15
23
31
37
39
43
v
Children face many important changes in the
first eight years of life, including different
learning centres, social groups, roles and
expectations. Their ability to adapt to such a
dynamic and evolving environment directly
affects their sense of identity and status within
their community over the short and long term.
In particular, the key turning points in childrens
lives – such as graduating’ from kindergarten
to primary school or going through a culturally
specific rite of passage – provide challenges
and opportunities for learning and growth on
multiple levels.
This paper provides a review of the major
perspectives in research on early childhood
transitions and reveals the predominant areas of
focus in both academic and professional studies,
as well as important neglected viewpoints and
study populations. Beginning with a broad and
inclusive definition of the topic, the authors
provide an overview of early childhood
transitions research, highlighting the underlying
assumptions that informed the studies. They
assess concepts in the developmental theory that
preceded transitions research as well as in the
logic that determines how transitions are
structured. More recent approaches are
examined, including systems theories and the role
of children as active participants in transitions.
Several examples in this review show how
multidisciplinary collaboration and culturally
sensitive interventions can result in better
participation of both parents and children in
crucial early childhood transitions. Citing the
need to harmonise early childhood education
and care programmes with local education
practices, the authors stress the value of
greater transparency in the creation of policy
and programming for children, in order to
identify potentially limiting assumptions.
Broadening and diversifying perspectives on
transitions can lead to more integrated and
culturally relevant rights-based early childhood
programmes worldwide.
Executive summary
1
Introduction
Transitions are now recognised as central to
young childrens experiences and well-being,
as well as a powerful integrative framework for
research. This review surveys major conceptual
tools that shed light on different aspects of early
childhood transitions. The objectives are
twofold:
1) to review major research perspectives
on early childhood transitions and
2) to identify
significant trends (and gaps) in the knowledge
base of scholarly as well as professional studies.
The findings of the review point to the value of
widening perspectives on transitions in order
to inform integrated and contextualised child-
focused policy and programming.
The major purpose of the review is to assist the
Bernard van Leer Foundation and its partner
organisations in their efforts to foster realisation
of universal child rights in culturally sensitive
ways. By linking concepts, theories and practice,
the review offers an accessible resource that will,
we hope, have wide appeal for both researchers
and practitioners concerned with early childhood
transitions.
Following the working definition of General
Comment
7 to the Convention on the Rights of
the Child,
1
early childhood’ is understood as the
period below the age of
8 (Committee
on the Rights of the Child,
2005: 2). Early years
transitions research and policy is especially
important to realising the rights of young
children, as this phase of life is generally
acknowledged as a period of accelerated and
intense change, usually involving multiple
developmental, social, and (for increasing
numbers of children), institutional transitions,
each of which has implications for current well-
being and long-term outcomes.
The term ‘transitions’ has a variety of meanings
that are not readily captured in a single
definition. The review takes an inclusive
understanding of transitions as its starting
point. We aim to situate different approaches
within relevant theoretical frameworks in order
to highlight the underlying assumptions about
childhood and child development that inform
them. One generic definition would be that
transitions are key events and/or processes
occurring at specific periods or turning points
during the life course. They are generally linked
to changes in a persons appearance, activity,
status, roles and relationships, as well as
associated changes in use of physical and social
space, and/or changing contact with cultural
beliefs, discourses and practices, especially
where these are linked to changes of setting and
in some cases dominant language. They often
involve significant psychosocial and cultural
1
In 2005, General Comment 7 arose out of the Committee of the Rights of the Child’s concern about the lack of information
being offered about early childhood and a perceived need for a discussion on the broader implications of the Convention on the
Rights of the Child for young children. Through General Comment
7, the Committee wishes to encourage recognition
that young children are holders of all rights enshrined in the Convention and that early childhood is a critical period for the
realisation of these rights.
adjustments with cognitive, social and emotional
dimensions, depending on the nature and causes
of the transition, the vulnerability or resilience
of those affected and the degrees of change and
continuity of experiences involved.
In practice, transition concepts are often used
in much more differentiated and specific ways,
for example, in terms of vertical and horizontal
‘passages (Kagan and Neuman,
1998: 366).
Vertical transitions may be thought of as key
changes from one state or status to another,
often associated with ‘upward’ shifts (e.g.,, from
kindergarten to primary school; from primary
to secondary school, etc.). General Comment
7
as well as most research conducted within the
field of education studies is primarily concerned
with the kinds of vertical shifts produced within
the context of formal schooling. Indeed, in
many secularised societies the significant
transitions of early childhood are intimately
linked with educational institutions (Arnold et
al.,
2007: 2; UNESCO 2006: 14).
Less attention has been paid by educational
researchers to what are sometimes referred to
as education-associated transition processes’
(Fabian and Dunlop,
2007: 11), those less-formal
changes in childrens lives and routines that
occur outside institutional settings. Nonetheless,
these apparently ‘peripheral’ changes may in fact
crucially and continuously shape childrens
experiences and pathways, and be very central’
in shaping childrens life trajectory and well-
being. Indeed, these key social transitions during
the life course have been routinely studied by
anthropologists working within a very different
paradigm and most often within non-western
societies where childhood has until recently
been less decisively shaped by age-related
institutions and laws. Social transitions are just
as significant, seen as critical thresholds and
often referred to as ‘rites of passage’, a term
originally introduced by Van Gennep
(1960).
These transitions are rooted in local belief
systems and typically expressed through rituals
(e.g., circumcision, first communion) that may
or may not be organised by formal institutions
(Morrow,
2003: 268).
Horizontal transitions are less distinctive than
vertical transitions and occur on an everyday
basis. They refer to the movements children
(or indeed any human being) routinely make
between various spheres or domains of their
lives (e.g., everyday movements between home
and school or from one caretaking setting to
another). These structure childrens movement
across space and over time, and into and out of
the institutions that impact on their well-being.
Research on early institutional transitions has
tended to conceptualise transitions as a one-
point’ event (e.g., first day at primary school).
However, since the late
1990s, research directions
have been shifting, with more studies under-
standing transitions as a multi-layered and
multi-year process, involving multiple
continuities and discontinuities of experience
(Petriwskyj, Thorpe, Tayler,
2005: 63).
Nonetheless, transitions research continues to
focus largely on modern educational institutions
in Europe, the
USA, Australia and New Zealand,
2
3
with major research gaps on transition practices
in less-industrialised contexts. To anticipate
the conclusions of the review, more studies are
needed to explore the impact of educational
programmes that reflect and adapt to childrens
diverse local environments. At the same time,
studies into childrens educational transitions
increasingly emphasise the need to make more
explicit the link between socio-cultural contexts
and childrens school transition experiences
(e.g., Yeboah,
2002).
This review explores how a range of transitions
concepts and research can inform rights-based
early childhood policies and practices. It does
not focus on policy and programme develop-
ments per se, but on underlying conceptualisa-
tions about transitions in early childhood. The
review emerged in response to the growing need
for orientation among the myriad concepts and
theories in both child research and practice:
“[P]eople often dismiss theoretical or pure
research as being of no consequence for
children and having no importance in the
‘real’ world. This attitude could not be more
incorrect. Good applied research depends upon
theoretical work both at the stage of developing
a research project and when results are being
analyzed. (Boyden and Ennew,
1997: 10)
The
UN Committee on the Rights of the Child
corroborates the importance of theory in
informing rights-based work with children:
“Theory and evidence from early childhood
research has a great deal to offer in the
development of policies and practices, as
well as in the monitoring and evaluation of
initiatives and the education and training of
all responsible for the well-being of young
children (
UNCRC et al., 2006: 53).
Overview
Chapter 1 begins by outlining developmental
concepts which underpin transition themes,
in particular those associated with the theories
of Jean Piaget and other ‘stage theorists. Their
ideas are highlighted early on because so much
transitions research builds on or reacts to core
developmental assumptions. Chapter
1 then
introduces socio-cultural perspectives on early
childhood transitions. These are distinguished
by their focus on how children learn by
interacting with their immediate socio-cultural
environments (e.g., caregivers, peers). This
emphasis has been elaborated by several
disciplines within the social sciences and is
increasingly mirrored in early child development
programmes around the world.
Chapter
2 examines the different ways in
which transitions are structured, drawing
attention to varying logics that can be employed
to mark transitions in early childhood.
Institutional settings often use biological age as
the criterion for readiness. By contrast, socio-
cultural transitions are often marked through
rites of passage, following the cultural and
economic reasoning of a given community.
Also, around the world children engage in
horizontal transitions as they move between
different domains of everyday life.
Introduction
4
Chapter 3 shifts to perspectives on transitions
that are informed by systems theories. These are
distinguished from socio-cultural approaches
by their greater emphasis on the links between
individuals, macro social processes and historical
changes. These approaches highlight the
linkages between children, their communities
and global societies and draw attention to the
importance of comprehensive programmes that
enable children to engage critically with the
demands of a changing environment.
Chapter
4 focuses on childrens active roles in
shaping their transition experiences, with
particular attention to the significance of peer
group relationships as a moderating influence on
transitions. The section then explores research
methods that may enable the implementation of
childrens right to participation within research
and programming in this area.
The final chapter discusses the findings of this
review, highlighting significant research strengths
and gaps of the various approaches presented,
followed by a glossary of key transitions
concepts discussed in the paper.
5
Conceptualisations of transition are almost
invariably underpinned by theories about
childrens development, especially as informed
by developmental psychology. Development is a
foundational concept for early childhood policy
and practice, and it is also central to realising
childrens rights. The United Nations Convention
of the Rights of the Child
(UNCRC) draws
heavily on the concept of development both as
a substantive right (Article
6) and as a standard
against which to protect children from harmful
experiences (e.g., Article
32) (Woodhead, 2005).
Developmental theories necessarily engage with
concepts of transition, even if not explicitly.
Development is all about processes of individual
growth, change and transformation, and it is
frequently conceptualised in terms of moving
through a sequence of age-approximate stages.
At the same time, ‘development’ is a very wide-
ranging concept, permitting multiple theoretical
interpretations, with each theory suggesting
different ways to understand personal transitions.
We begin by briefly summarising some features
of Jean Piaget’s constructivist theory’, which has
been most influential through the elaboration of
stages of human development. Other notable
stage theorists include Lawrence Kohlberg
(1981)
on moral development and Erik Erikson (1950)
on personal and social development.
Developmental stages as transitions
Developmental stage theory is epitomised by
Piaget’s ideas, especially as these have been
enthusiastically taken up by educational theorists
and curriculum planners. Broadly speaking,
early child development is seen as a natural and
universal process of progressive transformations
(or stages) in childrens physical, mental,
cognitive, socio-emotional and moral
competencies. These transformations are driven
by the interactions between maturational
processes and childrens progressive structuring
and restructuring of their experiences, as they
gradually acquire more sophisticated capacities
for thinking and reasoning. Stage theorists
were typically guided by the hypothesis that the
sequence of stages is invariant and universal, and
this prompted extensive cross-cultural research
during the
1960s and 1970s to compare childrens
capacities on Piaget’s tasks across diverse
cultural settings. Piaget
(1978) envisaged these
psychological stages as driven by a process of
equilibration. He suggested that children
develop schemata to represent their
understanding of the world, and that they try
to assimilate the world to these schemata until
too much external contradiction forces a change
and re-equilibration of their world view
(Lourenco and Machado,
1996: 149). The
implication of seeing child development as a
series of progressive psychological
transformations, from one stage to the next,
from infancy to maturity, is that these stages
become crucial reference points for discussing
optimal timing for transitions, e.g., from home
to pre-school or from more informal to more
formal curriculum.
Chapter 1: Development and transition
During the 20
th
century, Piaget’s early writing,
as well as partial readings of his work, were
popularised and globalised. This diffused version
of Piagetian theory was often stripped of the
subtleties and complexities of his original work.
Yet, it is the simplifications of the theory that
have fed into the predominant framework for
welfare and education programmes, as well
as child legislation (Boyden,
1997: 197). For
example, debates surrounding the concept and
assessment of childrens readiness for learning
and/or readiness for school are strongly fed by
developmental ideas. These debates are in turn
influential on beliefs about a child’s readiness
to make successful transitions. The concept of
readiness appeared in the educational literature
during the
1920s. Promoted by developmentalists,
readiness for learning was regarded as the level
of development at which the individual has the
capacity to undertake the learning of specific
material – usually the age at which the average
group of individuals has the specified capacity
(Good,
1973). By contrast, readiness for school
is a more finite construct, embracing specific
cognitive and linguistic skills. Irrespective of
academic domain, school readiness typically
sanctions standards of physical, intellectual
and social development considered sufficient
to enable children to fulfil school requirements
(Scott-Little et al.,
2006). Early specifications of
developmentally appropriate practice in early
childhood programmes were also strongly
informed by stage-based theories (e.g.,
Bredekamp,
1987).
The influence of developmental ideas cannot be
underestimated: “Developmental psychology can
be seen as a discourse which not only contributes
to the construction of our images of children
and our understanding of childrens needs, but
also to the construction and constitution of the
whole childhood landscape (Dahlberg, Moss,
and Pence
1999: 36). For example, under the
developmental paradigm the dialogue revolving
around young childrens needs and provision
rarely viewed them as rights-holders with their
own views and perspectives. Instead very young
children have often been perceived as objects
of benevolence and passive recipients of care
(
UNCRC et al., 2006: 31–32).
Although developmental stage theories were
for many decades the dominant framework for
understanding childrens transitions, especially
amongst progressive child-centred educationists,
a growing body of research and theory across the
social sciences contributed to a shift in the
academic perception of children and childhood.
One influential alternative to stage theory came
from within developmental psychology itself,
building on the ideas of Lev Vygotsky (e.g., Rogoff,
2003). Another influential strand of theory came
from the new sociology of childhood, which
has fuelled the critique of the developmental
paradigm itself (e.g., Qvortrup
1994, James and
Prout
1997, Woodhead, in press). One of the
main areas of critique has surrounded theoretical
positioning of children as human ‘becomings’
rather than human ‘beings’, in other words, as
competent and active participants in society
from birth (summarised by Uprichard,
2008).
General Comment
7 of the Committee on the
Rights of the Child has taken into account this
6
7
growing body of research and recognises that
currently more is known about the capacities
and the development of infants and young
children than was known during the
1980s when
the working group drafted the Convention:
“Research in the last decades has impressively
confirmed that children from an early age are
explorers with boundless curiosity and that they
are judicious decision makers and social actors
each with their own unique goals, interests and
ways to communicate feelings and intentions
(Doek, Krappmann and Lee,
2006: 32).
These new understandings of childrens active
participation in social activities call for an
approach to child development that emphasises
the plurality of developmental pathways and
childrens roles in influencing their own
development (Estep,
2002: 143).
Transitions as socio-cultural learning
processes
Socio-cultural learning refers to the diverse
ways in which caregivers and communities
enable children to achieve mastery of culturally
acknowledged and valued behaviour. While the
process of socio-cultural learning exists
everywhere, the goals of these learning processes
vary within communities and historical periods.
This perspective does not deny the significance
of universal maturational processes, but
encourages closer examination of the meaning
of ethnotheories
2
and the contexts that inform
childhood transitions and rites of passage.
As noted above, the origins of this approach are
in part to be found within social constructivist
(or socio-cultural) perspectives (Woodhead,
1998). Vygotskian theory breaks from traditional
developmental psychology by focusing on the
importance of social interaction. It emphasises
activity, rather than the individual, as the basic
unit of analysis. This more dynamic vision of
child development offers a relational view on
transitions. In this view, children are actively
involved in the timing and quality of their
transition experiences. Vygotskian socio-cultural
psychology has the advantage of recognising all
aspects of childhood as shaped by social,
cultural and economic processes. This also
applies to childrens environments, whether
these are within the home, the farm, or a pre-
school setting (Woodhead,
1999a: 9).
Like Piaget, Vygotsky viewed children as active
agents in their own environment, engaging with
the world around them, and in some senses,
creating for themselves the circumstances of
their own development. Where the two theorists
differ is in the emphasis given by Vygotsky to the
role of cultural and social processes in learning
and development. Vygotsky understands
learning as a process that results in development.
In this respect, he clearly differs from Piagetian
Development and transition
2
Ethnotheories represent emic views on childhood as well as beliefs about what activities are reasonable for children to carry
out and how these fit into the wider set of social practices. Interestingly, what transpires from different ethnographies on child-
rearing practices is the existence of similarities in cross-cultural ethnotheories with regard to the position of children within the
human life course, as in respect to major points of transitions during youth. At the same time, this research also underscores the
high degree of diversity in terms of developmental goals and socializing strategies (Boyden, Ling and Myers,
1998: 32-35).
8
approaches, which stress that a certain develop-
mental stage has to be reached in order to learn
(Feldman and Fowler,
197: 1999). The transition
between learning and development occurs in
the so-called ‘zone of proximal development’
(Vygotsky,
1978), referring to the distance
between the most difficult task a child can
perform without help and the most difficult task
s/he can do with support. It is therefore through
the instruction from teachers, adults and more
skilled peers that children learn and develop.
Post-Vygotskian researchers developed the idea
of scaffolding’ to capture the assistance children
receive from their peers and adult instructors in
reaching new developmental goals (Wood et al.,
1976). In order to scaffold a child, parents, teachers
and peers use tools and signs as mediators to
transmit knowledge and practical routines.
Developmental goals, as well as the mediating
tools, are all culturally defined (Estep,
2002: 152;
Mooney,
2000: 83–84). In a similar vein, the
concept of guided participation in cultural
activities highlights how children can learn
to think and to develop new skills and more
mature approaches to problem solving with
guidance from more skilled peers, siblings, and
adults (Rogoff et al.,
1998: 227).
Transitions can be understood as key moments
within the process of socio-cultural learning
whereby children change their behaviour
according to new insights gained through social
interaction with their environment. This chapter
explores frameworks that are informed by socio-
cultural theory, namely ‘developmental niche’
(Super and Harkness,
1986) and guided
participation (Rogoff,
1990). The section
concludes by looking at the significance of inter-
generational influences.
Developmental niche
The idea of a developmental niche’ refers to the
combination of:
1) caregivers’ belief systems
(ethnotheories) regarding child-rearing,
2) the
material conditions and, in particular, the
spatial arrangements, of child-rearing, and
3)
the actual practices of child-rearing. At the
centre of the model rests the individual child
(Super and Harkness,
1986: 552), and although
it is very family- and child-centred,
3
it does not
look at wider social effects (unlike ecological
models; see below). The three sub-systems of
the developmental niche represent the way
individual childrens worlds are arranged and
are related to the wider cultural environment. In
this view, children contribute to the construction
of their developmental niches through their
own expectations and through their interaction
with their caregivers.
The developmental niche’ approach has mostly
been used to study early child-rearing practices
in relation to local beliefs and customs,
3
‘The child’ is also at the centre of analysis of other conceptual models. For example, although Bronfenbrenner’s ecological
system theory focuses much more on social constraints than the developmental niche approach, ‘the child’ remains at the
centre of analysis.
9
Development and transition
Example: GEAR UP
Applied to formal transitions to institutions, the developmental-niche-approach encourages researchers
to examine settings, customs, and ethnotheories both at home and in care contexts in order to unveil
cultural differences between these spaces. For example, Harkness and colleagues
(2005) have been
using the concept to explore pathways and transitions between home and school of inner city children
in Hartford (Connecticut,
USA). Starting in 1999, researchers followed two cohorts of children in the
sixth and seventh grades in a pre-kindergarten through eighth grade school. This school took part in
the University of Connecticut’s
GEAR UP project, a federally funded program to support children in
completing high school and continuing their education. At the time of the intervention,
64% of the
students were Hispanic,
22% African American, and the others Caucasian, Asian or Native American.
67% came from non-English-speaking homes. (Harkness et al., 2005: 341–342)
First, researchers acquainted themselves with families involved in the project through home visits
and interviews which provided qualitative and quantitative indicators of children’s cultural and
educational backgrounds, parents’ concerns and their engagement with their child’s school. Exploring
children’s home and school developmental niches demonstrated that pupils experienced considerable
discontinuities between those two spaces.
After these initial assessments, researchers proposed interventions shaped by the developmental niche
framework. As the framework places emphasis on children within their families, it was assumed that
any intervention has to target children as well as their caregivers. Thus, the set-up of a mentoring
system became a crucial component of the
GEAR UP project. The intervention proved very helpful in
increasing students’ scholastic and social competence. Similar to the Vygotskian concept of ‘scaffolding’,
support of either official mentors and/or competent peers transpired to improve individual children’s
school performance. Moreover, mentors met with parents and thus fostered parental interest and
involvement in school matters. Furthermore, the
GEAR UP project initiated a variety of after-school
activities in order to improve the relationship between children, their caregivers and the school.
Through the participation of younger siblings, parents became increasingly involved in these
activities. Interestingly, researchers also came to understand that their previous involvement with
parents through home visits and interviews was actually an intervention in itself. It was only through
these home visits that many parents realised their opinions would be taken into account. Children
also seemed to welcome the presence of
GEAR UP project-related persons to their homes and their
participation to project-related activities reportedly increased as a result of the interviews and home
visits (Harkness et al.,
2005: 350).
illustrated by LeVine’s research amongst the
Gusii of Kenya (Le Vine et al.,
1994). He reports
that, traditionally, high birth and mortality
rates placed a premium on early nurturance,
with close physical contact, demand feeding
and sleeping next to the mother. This nurturant
style did not incorporate high levels of playful
stimulation; mothers remained aloof, with little
joint activity or verbal communication. At the
same time, managing a large family as well as
cultivating the fields put pressure on the mother
as caregiver and necessitated a significant
contribution from her children. The baby would
be entrusted to the care of an older sibling,
and by the age of
3 would already be expected
to carry out small domestic chores. Deference
to elders and obedience to instructions was
emphasised; praise offered sparingly. LeVine et
al. compare the Gusii infant’s experience with
a child in Boston, whose survival is virtually
assured and whose relationships are marked by
reciprocity and mutual responsiveness. Children
are provided with plenty of psychological space,
they are encouraged to assert their individuality,
and clashes of will are tolerated and in
moderation seen as a sign of healthy development
(as summarised in Woodhead,
1998). These very
different niches for early childhood are highly
significant in their own right, but they also have
very different implications for the transitions
children might make to different settings or
contexts, through migration, starting school,
etc.; these new settings represent a very different
developmental niche, governed by different
values, goals and expectations.
Guided participation
The concept of guided participation emphasises
both the active engagement of children in their
social world, as well as the role of adults and
peers in guiding children towards full
participation in culturally valued activities.
While the process of guided participation is
universal, it differs according to the degree of
communication between children and their
caregivers, as well as in the skills expected from
mature community members (Rogoff,
1990:
190). The guided participation concept expands
Vygotsky’s understanding of ‘zone of proximal
development’ (which focused mainly on cultural
mediation through language and literacy) by
highlighting the role of tacit forms of
communication and practical activities in
encouraging child development. Rogoff
emphasises that children are constantly engaged
in an appropriation of culture even when they
are seemingly passive (e.g., eavesdropping and
observation), as well as through active
participation: “Instead of viewing children as
separate entities that become capable of social
involvement, we may consider children as being
inherently engaged in the social world even from
before birth, advancing throughout development
in their skill in independently carrying out and
organising activities of their culture (Rogoff,
1990: 22).
Developmental transitions, within this
framework, relate to the gradual mastery of
cultural tools. This view contrasts with stage
10
11
theories, where transitions appear more like
once-for-all transformations in cognitive and
psychosocial functioning. The guidance of
culturally competent peers and adults as well as
the mediation of culturally meaningful symbols
allows children to become more confident in
their ability to perform culturally valued routines
and activities and in their acquired skills. These
‘repertoires of practice reflect deep-seated
cultural dispositions and are difficult to change:
“People’s repertoires of practice describe the
formats they are likely to employ in upcoming
situations, based on their own prior experience
in similar settings. Repertoires of practice are
highly constrained by people’s opportunities
and access to participate directly or vicariously
in settings and activities where particular formats
are employed” (Rogoff et al.,
2005: 27).
Inter-generational influences on transitions
Both developmental niche and guided
participation approaches highlight the role of
caregiver influence in cultural reproduction and
child development and well-being. Inter-
generational influences encompass more than
biological caregivers, and may come from
childrens cultural interactions with parents,
grandparents, aunts and uncles, teachers,
neighbours, religious leaders and other adults.
Those adults with whom children spend the
most time and/or whom they have fashioned as
their role models (or anti-role models) may
have greater impact on childrens orientations
and transitions; and who they spend the most
time with – at home, in institutional contexts, at
play, etc. – may alter over the course of childhood.
The impact of parents and family members on
childrens school outcomes was illustrated by
a study carried out on seven African countries
by Lloyd and Blanc (
1996, cited in Lloyd et al.,
2005). They found that variations in childrens
schooling outcomes could be explained by the
resources of the child’s residential household,
particularly the standard of living and the
education of the head of household. When
comparing households with similar resources,
children living in female-headed households
were found to fare better than children in male-
headed households in terms of school outcomes.
In many developing country contexts,
household relationships are characterised by the
interdependency of its various members, which
may be reinforced by deep-seated notions of
respect and obligation and by financial necessity.
Decisions around which childhood transitions
are important when, for which children exactly,
and who gets involved, are negotiated across
generations and reflect particular visions and
cultural representations of childhood and child
development. Whether the child is male or
female, or eldest or youngest, and his or her
phase in the life course will shape expectations
of who gets involved – and in which ways – at
key transition points. To illustrate, there has
been a relatively recent surge in interest in the
specific roles that fathers play in their childrens
development and well-being, though such
studies tend to be focused in industrialised
country contexts. For example, in the
US, it
was only in the
1970s that a scholarly interest in
fatherhood emerged and social policies targeting
fathers expanded, Marsiglio and colleagues note
Development and transition
12
in a review in the Journal of Marriage and the
Family (2000). Since then, research has moved
beyond focusing on the limited role of fathers as
‘breadwinners’ or in terms of their deficits (i.e.,
deadbeat dads’, absent fathers, concern with
‘female-headed households, etc.) with greater
focus on the positive impacts they have on their
childrens lives. Major changes in family life and
organisation have highlighted the diversity of
fatherhood and have opened a space for
examining fathers’ potentially unique
contribution to child development (Marsiglio,
p.
1174).
In their review, Marsiglio and colleagues pointed
to the
1990s as the decade in which US interest
in the study of fatherhood intensified, reflected
in the number of national surveys that added
questions to their instruments to capture fathers
involvement and experiences of fatherhood
(p.
1174). Survey examples include the a) Panel
Study of Income Dynamics, b) National Survey
of Labor Market Experience – Youth, c) National
Survey of Adolescent Males, d) National Survey
of Families and Households, and e) National
Survey of Family Growth. The Developing a
Daddy Survey project draws on six national
studies to study father involvement, including
a) the Early Head Start Evaluation – Fatherhood
Component, b) the Early Childhood
Longitudinal Study – Birth Cohort and c) the
Fragile Families and Child Wellbeing Study.
Though limited to survey studies, these efforts
represent an increasing interest in understanding
the distinct influences different adults – in this
case, fathers – have on the their childrens lives.
There is relatively less research on father’s
involvement in developing country contexts
(Engle and Breaux,
1998). Understanding
the different ways in which fathers, mothers,
grandparents, older siblings, etc. contribute
to childrens transitions – as decision-makers,
sources of material or emotional support, and as
role models (or ‘anti’-role models) – can inform
local programmes aimed to support childrens
transitions experiences and well-being.
Summary
This section began with an overview of
developmental stage theories that provided an
influential underpinning for understanding
major changes during childhood, and have in
turn been highly influential on policies and
practices surrounding childrens transitions.
Socio-cultural theories, which offer an alternative
perspective, have been illustrated through the
specific concepts of developmental niche’ and
guided participation. These concepts, along
with a brief review of inter-generational
influences, are strong reminders that cultural
values, belief systems and relationships shape
assumptions about child-rearing practices,
developmental goals, and the methods aimed
to reach them, as well as the basis upon which
childrens progress is assessed.
In General Comment 7, the
UN Committee on
the Rights of the Child reflected this awareness
of culturally diverse child-rearing goals and
practices and encouraged those working with
young children to “draw on beliefs and
13
Development and transition
Examples: Programmes that link formal education with the learning of culturally valued skills
The Grandmother Project (Senegal)
The Grandmother Project (www.grandmotherproject.org) is an international non-profit organisation
based in the
USA and Italy which was set up in 2004 in order to actively involve grandmothers
in community health and early childhood education programmes. The project acknowledges
grandmothers’ experience and contribution to child and family well-being as well as their exclusion
from programme models that ignore local culture (Ageways,
2007). It supports community-based
organisations and non-governmental organisations to learn grandmother-inclusive approaches and
plan and implement programmes that feature grandmothers as key actors. In southern Senegal, West
Africa, the Grandmother Project developed a booklet on the role of grandmothers in the local culture
for use in literacy classes and schools. It aimed to bridge the gap between young and old, and between
the domains of home and school. The project director, Judi Aubel, said, “Teachers tend to have a bias
against grandparents because they didn’t go to school. Even if your ultimate goal is to reach children,
you need first to work with teachers to change their attitude (p.
9).” In addition, older people involved
in the project reported an increase in self-confidence; as one grandmother said, “I have never seen a
book that talks about our role in society. It is true what it says that we do all that we can to ensure the
well-being of the family. But usually our role isn’t recognised.” (p.
9)
Alternative Basic Education for Karamoja
(ABEK) (Uganda)
ABEK is an early childhood programme funded by different organisations, including the Bernard van
Leer Foundation and Save the Children. It illustrates how the provision of children’s basic education is
adapted to community and livelihood practices. The programme aims to facilitate children’s transition
from informal to formal schooling in pastoral communities in Uganda. By adapting teaching to pastoral
lifestyles, the programme managed to change parental attitudes towards education. Importantly, the
programme respected the particularities of the community by teaching under trees at suitable hours
so that learning would not interfere with domestic chores. Also, curricula are taught by community
members. Importantly, teaching combines the transmission of formal knowledge (e.g. reading, writing,
numeracy, etc.) in relation to indigenous knowledge and culturally valued life skills (Chelimo,
2006: 36–37).
Opportunity for Poor Children
(OPC) (Mae Hong Son, Thailand)
OPC is a community-based organisation committed to the promotion of child rights and illustrates the
notion of `guided participation’ through the culturally valued work they do with migrant children.
OPC provides shelter, security and education for numerous Burmese migrant children between 5 and 15
years old who are living separated from their working parents in Mae Hong Son. Apart from schooling
and accommodation,
OPC provides students with culturally valued skills such as cooking and farming, as
the centre has a garden and rears pigs and chickens. The students work to tend the crops and animals,
learning as they do the skills under the guidance of more experienced peers and adults. In this way,
migrant children become familiar with organic subsistence farming of mushrooms, potatoes, garlic
and other crops, animal rearing (pigs) as well as the production of soy bean meal and tofu, which are
important basic foods. Teaching migrant children both literary and culturally valued skills proves to be
an important complement to education and may also provide them with prospects for future economic
gain and employment.
14
knowledge about early childhood in ways that
are appropriate to local circumstances and
changing practices, and respect traditional
values, provided these are not discriminatory
(art.
2), nor prejudicial to childrens health and
well-being (art.
24.3.), nor against their best
interests (art.
3)” (UNCRC, 2006: 38).
Furthermore, the Committee expanded in
General Comment
7 its definition of education
by insisting that every child has a right to
education, beginning from birth. In this sense,
education is being understood in a much
broader sense than schooling or pre-school, and
requires a comprehensive community effort
to support children through their early and
middle childhoods (Woodhead and Moss,
2007:
2). These concerns for implementing young
childrens rights in ways that are contextually
appropriate reinforce the importance of
understanding the local child development
circumstances, goals and available resources,
and differential involvement of family members
in key transitions, in order best to support
children as they experience key life changes.
15
If we understand transitions as key events or
processes of change over the life course, it is
important to make transparent the ways these
changes are defined and shaped in terms of
social structures and institutional processes. In
industrialised societies, childhood transitions
are often conceived as developmental processes
structured by educational institutions, for
example, as in the organisation of cohorts of
same-age children as first graders in school. In
this thinking, schooling is an assumed universal
feature of childhood, and biological age is widely
treated as a proxy for readiness, maturation and
competence. Yet, in many parts of the world
where birthdates are not recorded and schooling
still far from universal, biological age is not the
most important structuring factor for transitions
in childhood. Instead, social class or caste as well
as gender and birth order may be determinants
of childrens daily activities, life changes and
expectations for present and future development
(Woodhead, in press). Non-age-graded
perspectives on transitions may capture the
context of relevant passages, as well as childrens
strategic actions for adaptation during these
stages. Such a perspective would, on the one
hand, pay attention to one-time and usually
‘irreversible passages (e.g., circumcision, first
entry into school), as well as transitions that
occur on a regular basis (e.g., the horizontal
movements between primary school, home and
farming fields).
This section begins by looking at the structure
of institutional transitions, with particular
attention to the ways childrens age and their
gender function as social markers shaping the
way they experience and negotiate educational
settings. Next, two conceptual frameworks are
introduced which consider transitions as
culturally anchored movements between
periods and spheres of life. These vertical and
horizontal movements are respectively captured
by the concepts of rites of passage’ (e.g., first
school day) and ‘border crossings’ (e.g., daily
movements between home and school).
Age and gender in institutional
transitions
There is increasing awareness that inflexible,
institutional school structures with fixed age-
grade systems and associated aged-linked
curricula and assessment systems do not respect
the diversity of childrens developmental
pathways, nor the variations in their evolving
capacities (Lansdown,
2005). Although the
political momentum towards Education for
All has resulted in greater numbers of young
children enrolling in pre-schools and schools,
the timing of institutional transitions in early
childhood varies across countries and regions
according to how the primary school system is
organised at the local level. Many other socio-
cultural, institutional and economic factors
influence views on admission ages, and on the
organisation of age and gender cohorts. In some
countries, the transition from pre-school to
primary school may occur as early as age
4,
whereas in other places, children experience this
Chapter 2: The structure of transitions
16
transition around age 7. In some countries,
early education and care programmes are
seen as an integral first stage within the school
system; while in other countries, they are a
separate (and frequently diverse) sector
(Woodhead and Moss, 2007: 44).
Despite this awareness, child-focused policy
and practice generally conceive of childhood
transitions as appropriately timed achievements
of developmental milestones within educational
institutions. For example, within the field of
early childhood education, the term transition
is mostly used to define the move from one year
group to the next or from one school to another,
within formal educational settings (Fabian and
Dunlop,
2002: 3).
Age defines’ stages of childhood more
powerfully in Western societies, with annual
birthday parties commonplace for many children,
and school entry determined by date of birth.
This trend has been globalised through initiatives
such as Education for All. Nonetheless, in many
communities, age since birth is not recorded
and people often refer to relative seniority as
the measure of development, or link their birth
to particular historical events affecting their
community (Rogoff,
2003: 154). For example,
in Burundi and Tanzania, there are six different
names to refer to phases within childhood, with
transitions between them marked by the gradual
assumption of new responsibilities within their
families and communities (Eggers,
1997: 143;
Morrow,
2003: 272).
Indeed, channelling childrens transitions into
biologically timed processes through educatio-
nal systems is a relatively recent phenomenon.
Concern for the timing of childhood transitions
originated in the
USA and the UK with the
introduction of child labour laws and compulsory
schooling at the end of the
19
th
and beginning
of the
20
th
centuries (Cunningham, 1991: 194;
Cunningham and Viazzo, 1996: 6; Zelizer, 1985: 6).
Child labour laws aimed at curbing childrens
entry into the workforce and the establishment
of a standard school entry age facilitated the
organisational thinking surrounding pupils’
transition through school grades in terms of
‘batches, ‘cohorts’ or ‘peer groups. While grade
progression has generally been age-based in
education systems (e.g., the
UK), in many others
progression has been determined by achievement,
and grade repetition has been common. As
noted earlier, debates surrounding school
admission and progression became linked with
concerns for ‘readiness for school, developmental
appropriateness of curricula and ‘retardation in
learning.
Policy debates around age of school transitions
continue to this day. For example, in the
UK
the so-called ‘birth date effect’ has been widely
debated since the
1970s. Put simply, discussions
revolve around the question whether or not
summer-born children are disadvantaged at the
point of transition to school because they attend
pre-primary education for a shorter period
of time as well as being less mature than their
autumn-born peers (e.g., Bell and Daniels
1990).
Research literature surrounding organisation
of school starting ages is extensive, premised on
cultural conventions surrounding the labelling
of children as ‘pre-school’, ‘kindergarten,
‘reception class’, ‘first grade’, and so on.
17
Childrens chronological age has become a
powerful social marker shaping childrens lives
in the modern world, linked as it is to ideas
about stages and developmental readiness.
But age-related constructs do not function in
isolation. Like adults, children are complex
social beings who, from the time they are born,
take on identities based on multiple markers
including gender, ethnicity, class or caste, etc.
as well as age (Brooker and Woodhead,
2008).
In some communities, gender constructs are
virtually inseparable from age constructs in
determining for example, what is viewed
appropriate for a
5-year-old girl versus a
5-year-old boy. In the past, as well as in some
contemporary societies, gender was a major
factor determining whether children attended
school, as well as the kinds of schooling,
curriculum and teaching considered appropriate.
In the same way, just as many childhood rites of
passage are gender-specific, childrens experiences
of institutional transitions may also be shaped
by their gender. This isnt just a question of
cultural attitudes shaping adults’ expectations
and behaviour towards children. Children
themselves appropriate and negotiate gender
identity from an early age, especially through
their interactions with their peers, at pre-school
and elsewhere in their social worlds
(MacNaughton,
2000; Danby and Baker, 1998).
Especially as children mature and near puberty,
differences between boys and girls may become
heightened. Classrooms can be contexts where
stereotypical gender differences are reinforced
by teachers and peers, even where official
policies emphasise equal opportunities. These
problems may be amplified in low-resourced
schools in many developing country contexts,
where multi-grade, mixed-gender classrooms
are common. For example, in a study of the
abuse of girls in African schools, Leach et al.
(2003: viii) reported that schools in Ghana and
Malawi are a breeding ground for potentially
damaging gendered practices, the influence
of which will stay with pupils into adult life.
According to this report, “sexual aggression goes
largely unpunished, dominant male behaviour
by both pupils and teachers is not questioned,
and pupils are strongly encouraged to conform
to the gender roles and norms of interaction
which they observe around them. The latter is
also evidenced in resource-poor contexts, where
teachers often require pupils to carry out menial
tasks that are assigned in ways that reinforce
gender differences. Girls, for example, may be
asked to clean floors and toilets and fetch water,
while boys are required to carry bricks and cut
grass. Such practices may be less prevalent in
classrooms of very young children, but intensify
with age.
As an example, Jha and Kelleher (
2006: 92)
describe research aimed to explain boys
underachievement in Jamaica. The authors
point out that while gender parity indices have
improved for girls in recent years, boys are
underperforming, particularly at the secondary
level and in their progression to post-secondary
and tertiary education (p.
82). They describe
how local concepts of masculinity and
socialisation processes that foster greater
supervision of girls and less supervision of boys
have led to a lowering of boy’s self-esteem and
alienated them from the values of high academic
achievement.
The structure of transitions
Social transitions as rites of passage
The expression ‘rite of passage’ is widely used in
everyday language, as a way to describe significant
transition events during the life course. The
concept was introduced nearly a century ago
by Arnold van Gennep in his book Les rites de
passage
(1908).
4
Van Gennep was primarily
interested in the sequence of rites as markers of
life changes such as birth, name giving, maturity
and death. He was convinced that human
development consists of a series of passages that
are universal in form and cultural in content
(Hockey,
2002: 212).
According to Van Gennep, transitions are
dynamic processes which follow a threefold
sequential pattern: First, preliminal rites (‘rites
of separation from a previous ‘world’), second,
liminal or threshold rites (performed during the
transitional stage) and third, postliminal rites
marking an individual’s re-incorporation into
the world with a new status (Van Gennep,
1960:
21). These ideas were taken up by Victor Turner
who was particularly interested in liminality
as the phase when persons are “betwixt and
between structured stages of their life course
(Turner,
1969: 95). Because of the focus on
different stages, a rites of passage’-perspective
examines the whole process of transition, not
just specific marker events referred to in
everyday usage.
Rites of passage often refer to shifts in social
status and indicate readiness (or social
expectations) to take on new responsibilities.
They are frequently related to an individual’s
‘social age, rather than to their biological age.
In other words, the timing of cultural rites of
passage may depend on a variety of factors such
as socio-economic class or caste status, gender
and birth order, and will be strongly shaped by
the extent of modernisation and secularisation
of a society (Van Gennep,
1960: 66–67).
Young children are often at the centre of rites of
passage marking shifts in their status within the
community, both in terms of essential
personhood (i.e., becoming ‘fully human’) and
as social beings. It may not be important that a
very young child experiencing a rite of passage
is relatively unaware of the status change, as
these events are essentially about social
participation, recognition and affirming old
and new relationships.
Rites of passage in early childhood vary across
contexts in scale and formality, and in some
cases, such as the American ‘baby shower’, which
is usually celebrated either shortly before
childbirth or a few weeks after the child is born,
may be as much about affirming the mother’s
status as it is about ‘welcoming’ the baby. In the
Peruvian Andes, the relationship between the
newborn and their social world is at the fore
of the unuchakuy ritual, which introduces the
child to godparents and to a sacred mountain
believed to be a force of protection throughout
the child’s life (Bolin,
2006: 14). Ceremonial
naming is another way of marking transition in
18
4
The book was translated into English in 1960.
19
the early years; among the Maasai, this happens
for boys and girls at around 6 months of age
when they also get their first hair cut (embarnoto
e nkerai/enkidunkoto e nkaran). Among some
Jewish families, boys third birthdays are marked
by the upsherin ceremony, which involves the
ritual first haircut and is also meant to signal
their initiation into formal religious education.
In Burma, boys transition to adolescence is
related to their Buddhist initiation. This
comprises a formal ceremony followed by a
temporary withdrawal into monastic life while
wearing a yellow robe. Destitute households are
often unable to save the money for this
ceremony; therefore, it is not uncommon for
poor boys to make this transition experience late
or sometimes not at all. In contrast, some boys
are initiated at a relatively young age when their
older sisters are due for their own ceremonies
which must coincide with that of their brothers,
and without which the girls cannot enter into
marriage (Spiro,
1982: 234–235).
In Tanzania, the timing of traditional rites of
passage has been changing due to modern
medical technologies and compulsory schooling.
Circumcision ceremonies, for example, are often
conducted at the beginning of holidays, so as not
to interfere with schooling. Nevertheless, the rite
continues to convey traditional initiation messages
about making each other pregnant’ which some
children apparently put into practice following
initiation training (Morrow,
2003: 272–273).
The rites of passage’ framework has also been
applied to formal institutional transitions by
Lam and Pollard (
2006) in their study of
childrens transition to kindergarten in Hong
Kong. They used a holistic approach, integrating
rites of passage concepts with those from
Vygotskian socio-cultural theory. They also
draw on the concept of pupil careers to explore
positive outcomes of transitions (Woods and
Pollard,
1988). They identify three components
of ‘pupil careers’:
1) patterns of formal (academic)
and informal (social) outcomes,
2) strategic
action, and
3) an evolving sense of domain-
identity. Strategic action refers here to childrens
own contributions to becoming members of the
new school culture (e.g., through interaction
with friends). Domain-identity refers, in the
case of school, to childrens understanding of
themselves as pupils’. Although children are
physically part of and present in the school
system on a nearly daily basis, it may take
several weeks or months for them to grow into
their new domain identity’ or pupil role (Lam
and Pollard,
2006: 135–136).
The authors use rites of passage’ to describe the
phases of childrens transition to kindergarten,
drawing on socio-cultural theory to discern
how competent members at home and in
kindergarten (e.g., parents, teachers, experien-
ced peers) guide children with cultural media-
tion tools (communication, play, routines, etc.)
through their transition process. The preliminal
stage relates to childrens separation from their
caregivers at home as they learn to be pupils in
a classroom. The liminal stage starts with
transition practices or programmes that
inaugurate children into becoming pupils. This
phase entails a spatial passage (from home to
The structure of transitions
20
kindergarten, for example) as well as a status
passage (integrating pupil with child status).
The liminal transition process of learning to
become a pupil is characterised by ambiguity
and transformation, as children are not yet
fully integrated into the new pupil status. The
postliminal stage is reached at the end of the
transition process when children have adapted
their new pupil identity (Lam and Pollard,
2006:
131–132).
William Corsaros notion of priming events
also has a ritual character, echoing aspects of
rites of passage theory. Priming events are
interactive and symbolic activities that allow
children and their social environment to
contribute actively to their experiences of
change.
5
For example, in their ethnographic
study of a school setting in Modena, Italy,
Corsaro and Molinari show how priming events
at the end of kindergarten are public and widely
attended gatherings where children, their families,
and local politicians and other community
members join in the events. Such a public ritual
represents a break from everyday routine and is
meant to signal to the children (and others) that
it is time for them to move on. For parents and
the organisers of these events, there is a civic
pride in celebrating the lives of the children and
to see them growing up and becoming more
involved members of society. Also, for younger
siblings, participating in these events may be a
kind of priming activity that makes them
anticipate the moment when it will be their turn
to be in the spotlight (Corsaro and Molinari,
2005: 18).
Daily transitions as border crossings
The concept of rites of passage normally refers
to the cultural marking of once-for-all life
course transitions, sometimes also called
‘vertical transitions’, to contrast with ‘horizontal
transitions’ that occur on a daily and even
momentary basis (Kagan and Neuman,
1998).
Sue Campbell Clark (
2000) introduced the
notion of ‘border crossing’ to describe adults’
routine movements backwards and forwards
between home and the workplace, but this
concept can equally be applied to young
childrens daily experiences of moving between
home, pre-school and other everyday settings.
The concept provides a framework to describe
how borders are controlled and managed and
how individuals integrate and separate different
domains of their daily lives. Campbell Clark
underscores the role of supportive communication
in minimising cross-domain conflicts that may
arise when individuals frequently move between
two very different worlds (p.
764).
Indeed, research on early childhood transitions
suggests that, similar to adult border crossers,
young children experience identity shifts when
moving between domains. Identity shifts may
comprise changes in roles, dress and behaviour,
as well as activities and ways of communicating.
Border crossers can be either peripheral or
5
See also Chapter 4.
21
central participants to a domain. Central
participants have internalised the culture
and values of a domain, which allows them
to successfully perform the activities that are
valued within it and to interact with other
central domain members. Balance between two
domains is best achieved when border crossers
manage to identify with their roles in both
spaces (Campbell Clark,
2000: 759–761). It is
therefore important to examine the complexity
and the relationships between contexts,
expectations, and subjectivities. These concerns
draw attention to issues of home-school
cooperation as well as continuity issues when
children change from one educational setting to
another (Woodhead and Moss,
2007).
The structure of transitions
Example: Children Crossing Borders
Children Crossing Borders is a cross-cultural and multi-national study currently being conducted in five
countries (England, France, Germany, Italy and the United States) over a three-year period (
2006–9).
The study focuses on how immigrant children are being served by their early childhood care and
education (
ECCE) systems and what their parents want for their children in ECCE settings. It follows the
approach taken by Tobin, Wu and Davidson in their study `Preschools in Three Cultures.’ The method
entails creating videotapes of typical days in classrooms of four-year-olds in
ECCE settings in the study
countries. Key stakeholders (including parents, teachers, administrators, childhood education experts
and policy-makers, etc.) in each of the study countries are shown the same set of videos in order to
highlight similarities and differences in
ECCE systems and to encourage dialogue and debate. The
study’s link to ‘borders’ and ‘domains’ is clear. On the one hand, it addresses issues of conflict and
continuity in relation to differences between family domains and
ECCE settings. On the other hand, the
study also points to the cultural borders that immigrant families and children face as
ECCE settings may
be the first context in which they confront such differences. (See www.childrencrossingborders.org)
Example: RICA Project
Evidence from the RICA Project (2005–2006) on successful transitions to first grade in Nicaragua
highlights the effectiveness of working towards the interconnectedness of different domains. The
project consisted on the one hand of interventions aimed to bring school physically and psychologically
closer to children’s homes. On the other hand, project interventions addressed teachers in raising
awareness about children’s needs. Moreover, the programme focused on the expansion of pre-school
and primary school coverage in previously unattended zones, monthly support sessions to stimulate
parental participation in schools, dissemination of knowledge on child development among community
leaders, and awareness-raising workshops for teachers and headmasters on the particular needs of
children from disadvantaged sectors. Programme outcomes have been rewarding. After one year,
children’s attendance of pre-school during four of five days increased from
65% to 94%. Moreover,
pre-school children’s performance increased with
56% achieving the expected development level in
reading, writing and arithmetic, compared with
35% in the same schools during the previous year (Save
the Children
US, 2007).
22
Summary
This chapter has pointed to three different ways of
structuring transitions. First, we highlighted the
way transitions are structured by chronological
age, argued to be a historically recent practice
that originated in Western societies within the
context of the institutionalisation of education
at the beginning of the
20
th
century. Despite the
global importance of age as a transition marker,
especially within formal educational settings,
it is important to bear in mind that there also
exist other logics which structure transitions.
Gender has a powerful influence on all aspects
of childhood, although how much and in what
specific ways it shapes transitions varies between
families, communities and societies.
Social transitions, or rites of passage, mark
movements from one social status to another,
e.g., from child to adolescent or from pre-school
to primary school pupil. Border crossing theories
look at the borders children are crossing in their
daily lives and ask how children integrate
experiences in different everyday settings (e.g.,
school and home). Rites of passage theory draws
attention to the importance of the so-called
‘liminal period’. During this phase of the
transition experience, children are uprooted from
their previous environment (e.g., kindergarten)
without yet having fully adapted to their new
setting (e.g., primary school). It is during this
phase of transitions that interventions may
be most successful in influencing childrens
pathways.
Border crossing theories are based on the
assumption that various borders exist between
childrens different life domains, and that
breaking down the border’ between the domain
of home and school may benefit childrens
learning. Policy-makers and practitioners would
be encouraged to find ways for caregivers to
engage effectively in their childrens education
and for educators to better understand the
way childrens home environments shape their
school experiences. This view highlights the
need for greater understanding of the various
barriers that prevent caregivers from engaging
with their childrens education and raises the
question of the extent to which parents and
communities may benefit from an increased
sense of ownership of their childrens care and
educational institutions.
23
Childrens transitions are usually defined in
terms of the immediate contexts and practices
that shape their lives, notably in home, pre-
school and school settings. Systemic approaches
recognise that childrens experiences of transition
are embedded in wider social structures and
processes. For example, early childhood is
widely recognised as the period when the most
intensive care is needed in order to ensure
young childrens well-being, health, learning and
play. Within childrens immediate environment,
primary caregivers and peers have a pivotal
role in guiding young children through early
life transitions. Yet, when experiencing stress
through modern lifestyles, economic hardship,
conflict and other adversities, primary caregivers
may not always be in the capacity to provide
sufficient care and support for children.
This section explores ecological theory
(Bronfenbrenner,
1979, 1986) and life course
theory (Elder,
1994) as two conceptual models
that inform empirical research and practice on
childrens transitions within a wider social
context. These theoretical tools are helpful for
assessing lifetime implications of experiences of
risk and poverty in early childhood and
monitoring impacts of institutional structures
and relationships. Because of their focus on
wider structural influences, these theories
complement socio-cultural approaches, which
are more concerned with childrens immediate
environment.
Ecological theory
Ecological frameworks offer a comprehensive
approach for the study of transitions. Like
socio-cultural perspectives, ecological approaches
recognise childrens immediate experiences in
context, but also capture patterns of interaction
between individuals, groups and institutions
as they unfold over time (Rimm-Kaufmann
and Pianta,
2000: 500). They have the benefit of
capturing how, for instance, transitions on the
macro level (e.g., economic depression) impact
on children via parental unemployment
(exosystem) with consequences for the
household microsystems with which children
directly engage.
Psychologist Urie Bronfenbrenner elaborated
the ecological approach to human development
in the
1970s. It is informed by systems theory,
which underscores the interactions of (ever-
changing) individuals within the context of
their (ever-changing) environments. In his
research, Bronfenbrenner was mostly concerned
with an individual’s position in wider ecological
systems and how, for example, external
influences affect the capacity of caregivers to
foster the healthy development of children. He
perceived four aspects of the ecology in which
children grow up: microsystems, mesosystems,
exosystems, and macrosystems. ‘Microsystems
relate to childrens experiences and interactions
with peers, teachers and caregivers in everyday
Chapter 3: Transitions within systems
24
settings, at home, school, child care centre, etc.
‘Mesosystems’ are the relation between these
different microsystems, e.g., the complementary
and/or conflicting practices and belief systems
at home and at school and the informal/formal
communications between parents and teachers.
Bronfenbrenner argued that any setting involves
direct or indirect relations with other settings.
Therefore, analysis of mesosystems focus on
questions related to the shifts of settings and
roles individuals experience during ecological
transitions – for instance, whether children
enter a new setting alone or with familiar peers
or what kind of information children and their
parents receive before embarking on major
transitions. ‘Exosystems are areas of social life in
which children do not themselves participate,
but which nonetheless impact on their lives
and well-being through interconnections with
microsystems. For example, parental work
settings and practices are usually physically
separate from the settings children inhabit.
Their parents make daily ‘border-crossings’
from home to work, just as their children make
‘border crossings’ from home to pre-school or
school. But these exosystems impact on children,
insofar as, for example, physical proximity,
hours and conditions of work, etc., constrain
parents availability to care for children at home,
accompany them to pre-school, and so on. Note
that parental work patterns may be defined as
exosystems in industrialised countries, but this
may not be the case for agricultural communities,
where boundaries between children and their
caregivers’ work settings and activities may be
less sharply defined. Religious settings may also
form an exosystem until children go through a
rite of passage that grants them full participation
to sacred spaces and rituals (e.g., Ridgely Bales,
2005).
The influence of exosystems on children should
not be underestimated. Within systems theory
there exists concern for the organisation of
caregivers’ work as well as community
organisations and the capacity of public policy
to shape these systems in ways that are supportive
for child development. On Bronfenbrenner’s
original formulation, macrosystems relate to
the dominant beliefs and the organisation of
pervasive institutions that shape the cultural
settings in which children develop. Macrosystems
link with exosystems, as caregivers’ economic
activities are underpinned by policies, laws and
regulations.
Another feature of ecological theory concerns
childrens role in their development. In this view,
children develop through everyday interactions
with their caregivers and with other children,
symbols and objects in a given context. As
participants both within microsystems and at the
exosystem intersections between microsystems,
(i.e., transitions and border crossings), children
are not only influenced by their environment
but actively change it (Bronfenbrenner,
1986).
Although initially neglected, Bronfenbrenner’s
later work pays increasing attention to the
temporal dimension of transition experiences.
Thus, when systems-theory-inspired researchers
speak of chronosystems, they acknowledge the
historical context of their studies and examine
how historical changes impact upon transitions
in individual and community lives.
25
Ecological theory has been very influential as an
underpinning framework for the study of early
childhood, with important implications for the
study of transitions. But it also has limitations,
especially when systems are oversimplified and
reified. For example, while the identification
of multiple interacting systems is conceptually
elegant, there is a risk of objectifying boundaries
and assuming internal sub-system coherence,
(especially when represented by the classic
onion diagram). Each actor’s experiences
of their ecology will be different. Childrens
perspectives may be very different from adults,
which may be overlooked by an outside observer’s
attempt to model a singular ecology of child
development’ (Bronfenbrenner,
1979). Their
experiences of settings, relationships and
activities may be more dispersed and changing
than the concept of a microsystem implies,
especially where family relationships are
fractured, conflictual and even dysfunctional.
These complexities are highlighted by studies of
how household and family formation, breakdown
and reconstitution impact on childrens relation-
ship building, loss and adjustment as well as
their domestic arrangements and well-being
(e.g., Hagan et al.,
1996; Smart and Neale, 1999).
Another caution regarding ecological theory
relates to the way the model typically positions
the child at the centre of multiple nested
systems. While centring on the singular child
may be desirable from a social policy and child
rights perspective, it does not reflect the
multiple priorities of many of the systems in
which children participate, nor does it recognise
the competing priorities of adults with power
over their lives. For example, while being child
centred’ is ostensibly the raison d’être for child-
focused services, the child is but one of the
priorities within family settings, and the child
may be a marginal member of some community
systems. The interactions between individual,
social, economic, political and cultural processes
is also at risk of being overlooked within
Bronfenbrenner’s original formulation, especially
where individual and larger’ contexts are viewed
as separate entities organised in hierarchical
fashion of organising ‘larger contexts’
(macrosystems) in relation to smaller’ ones
(Rogoff,
2003: 45–46). Identifying cultural
context as part of macrosystems draws attention
away from the central role of cultural beliefs,
goals and practices in mediating childrens
experiences and activities at every level.
Despite these cautions, many researchers have
found Bronfenbrenner’s framework helpful
to explore experiences in early childhood in
general and transitions to kindergarten or
primary school in particular (e.g., Bohan-
Baker and Little,
2004; Johansson, 2007; Fabian
and Dunlop,
2002 and 2007; Rimm-Kaufman
and Pianta,
2000; Tudge et al., 2003; Tudge and
Hogan,
2005). For example, Rimm-Kaufman
and Pianta
(2000) conceptualise the ecology of
transitions to school with a particular focus
on the development of relationships over time,
underscoring the importance of context for
understanding childrens transition experiences.
Longitudinal research with repeated assessments
can contribute a lot to our understanding of
how changes in the contexts of childrens lives
impacts on childrens ability to make key life
Transitions within systems
changes. Furthermore, the complexity of the
linkage and the mutual shaping of contexts
would ideally require a combination of
qualitative and quantitative research methods.
Nevertheless, more studies are needed on the
link between local network-level interaction,
policy and programme participation, and
child development. Further research from this
perspective could address why some children
experience similar outcomes under different
policy conditions, or different outcomes under
the same policy conditions.
“The nature of change processes in the many
systems intervening between the macro and
individual levels has been under explored as
has the issue of how such processes mediate
effects of policy on development” (Yoshikawa
and Hsueh,
2001: 1888).
Yoshikawa and Hsueh insist that the role of the
family should neither be under- nor overestimated.
Family research using a variety of national and
other datasets shows that variations in parenting
style and home environments may explain as
much as half of the effect that household poverty
has on children. Understanding intra-household
dynamics (for example how resources and roles
are distributed among children in a given
household) could reveal the decision-making
processes that explain why some children make
certain transitions, while others do not.
There are other factors between policy and
household that impact on childrens transitions
(e.g., community influences and norms) and
these are usually poorly examined. For example,
there exists little research on how cultural
differences may influence the reception of
benefits (or respond to opportunities for
intervention in the early years). Also relatively
unexplored is the role of social networks within
communities and how these affect whether
or not families accept child-targeted welfare
(Yoshikawa and Hsueh,
2001: 1890).
Transitions and life course theory
Life course theory is closely related to the
ecological approach to human development.
The model understands human development
as a multi-level phenomenon, comprising
structured pathways through social institutions
and organisations to the social trajectories
of individuals (Elder,
1994: 5). More than any
other framework, this approach emphasises
that human development cannot be detached
from social history. On the one hand, historical
conditions shape the way children grow up. On
the other hand, history is produced through the
agency of children and adults (Elder,
1994: 5–6;
Ryder,
1965: 861).
Life course theory was built on evidence from
some of the earliest longitudinal cohort studies
in the
USA. The theory locates individual
trajectories in relation to massive social changes
such as migration, economic depression, and
armed conflict. Research in this area provides
evidence that the meaning of social change and
the capacity to cope with adversity strongly
depends on age status (Elder,
1994: 10). In his
study Children of the Great Depression (
1974),
26
27
Transitions within systems
Elder analysed data from two longitudinal studies
that had been conducted in California with
two cohorts of children born respectively at the
beginning and at the end of the
1920s. The
availability of this data allowed the assessment
of developmental outcomes throughout
childhood, adolescence and adulthood, and
provides robust evidence of the complex
interactions between individual characteristics,
household structures and the role of adversities.
As children of one sample were born eight years
earlier than those of the second sample, Elder
was able to compare the effects of economic
depression on two cohorts of children: those
who were adolescents when their families lost
income versus those who were still young
children at that time. The comparison showed
very different results for these groups. Children
whose caregivers became economically deprived
when they were adolescents seem to have gained
from the severe experience. Compared to non-
deprived young people, loss of family income
appeared to spur a sense of achievement, as
well as greater satisfaction in later life amongst
impoverished boys and girls. According to Elder,
the loss of income forced families to mobilise
human resources and provided mothers and
teenagers with new roles and responsibilities
which trained them in initiative, cooperation,
and responsibility. Yet, these favourable outcomes
were not shared by the younger cohort.
Compared to their peers from non-deprived
families, the cohort that experienced the
depression as very young children subsequently
had a lower school performance and showed
less stable work histories as well as observable
emotional and social difficulties until middle
adulthood. There were also important gender
differences. Negative outcomes were more
strongly displayed among boys. Their greater
vulnerability was very probably linked to their
father’s loss of livelihood, leading to
demoralisation and low self-esteem, which
transferred more to their sons than their
daughters, who were more likely to identify
strongly with their mother (Elder,
1974). In
short, Elderss study draws attention to the
impact of changing societies on developing lives.
It is particularly sensitive to the impact of
historical events (macrosystem changes in
ecological theory) on early childhood,
highlighting how these shape childrens
capacities to negotiate developmental transitions
and the consequences for their future agency
and life choices.
This research corroborates the evidence that
environmental variables impact differently on
children and encourages further research on the
balance of risk and protective factors that
contribute to childrens relative vulnerability
and resilience (e.g., Boyden,
2006; Boyden and
Mann,
2005; Hart, 2004; Mann, 2004; Yaqub,
2002). Studies of extreme deprivation have been
especially influential, notably in groups of
children in institutional care and orphanages.
These studies demonstrate that making an early
transition to an enhanced environment can
serve as a protective factor, with long-term
outcomes (e.g., Rutter et al.,
1998; Wolff and
Fesseha,
1999). Studies on school transitions
also examined how risk factors – accumulated
over a long-term period – cause academic
28
disengagement and eventual school drop-outs
(e.g., Alexander et al.,
1997; Punch, 2003, Watson-
Gegeo and Gegeo,
1992). Finally, there exists a
wealth of research evidence that well-resourced
ECCE programmes hold the potential of
supporting young children and their caregivers
in coping with adversities and improving their
prospects of successful school transitions.
Conversely, lack of professional and political
commitment to mobilise
ECCE action may
amplify the risks to young childrens well-being
(Jolly,
2007: 8; Woodhead, 2006: 11).
Summary
Ecological and life course theories highlight how
childrens transition experiences are embedded
in wider social systems. Ecological systems
theory draws particular attention to the
interconnections and mutual influences between
childrens and their caregivers social worlds.
These frameworks are valuable tools for
conceptualising the potential of early childhood
programmes and services to impact on childrens
life chances (Grantham-McGregor et al.,
2007).
Life course theory draws attention to the fact
that children relate and respond differently to
adversity. Not all children suffer from negative
repercussions of harmful experiences. Depending
on their social status, their age and their gender,
some children may even show resilience in the
face of adversity. Therefore, rather than relying
on the assumption of childrens ‘inherent
vulnerability’, it is important to evaluate the
impact of potential interventions in a more
Example: Young Lives
‘Young Lives’ is a 15-year four-country longitudinal study of childhood poverty that draws on ecological
and life course themes, with transitions as a particular focus. Begun in
2000, ‘Young Lives’ is funded
by the UK Department for International Development to follow the lives of
12,000 children growing
up in the context of poverty in Ethiopia, Andhra Pradesh (India), Peru and Vietnam. It was devised
to inform the Millennium Development Goals by increasing understanding of the causes, nature and
consequences of child poverty in order to provide a strong evidence base for child-focused policy. The
research combines quantitative and qualitative approaches to trace the life course trajectories of two
cohorts of children (separated by an eight-year age gap) within the contexts of their households and
communities. As subsequent rounds of data collection take place, studying two cohorts may enable
analyses into the way life course position mediates experiences of poverty and long-term outcomes
for well-being. Information is being collected every few years on each of the
12,000 children, their
caregivers and households, and the communities in which they are growing up. This will provide
insights into factors influencing change in three generations living in poverty, as some of the older
cohort of children (now aged
12 or 13) have become parents themselves. A life course approach is
necessary to understand the complexities of the inter-generational transfer of poverty and to explain
why individuals move into and out of poverty (see www.younglives.org.uk).
29
Transitions within systems
nuanced way, with regard to scale, timing and
focus (Walker et al.,
2007). These frameworks
also draw attention to structural hardships, as
these may put a strain on caregivers’ time, health
and resources. They emphasise the pivotal role
of programmes and interventions in
complementing primary caregivers efforts to
stimulate young childrens development
(
UNCRC et al., 2006: 44–48).
6
6
For example, article 18.3 recognises that in many parts of the world, caretakers are economically active in poorly paid
conditions and encourages measures to ensure that children of working parents benefit from childcare services and facilities to
which they are eligible (
UNCRC et al. 2006: 44).
31
Childrens participation in their transition
experiences has been addressed in traditional
academic studies in questions about their
activity and agency, and more broadly their role
in shaping their own childhoods (Woodhead,
2003). For example, Piagetian constructivist
paradigms within developmental psychology
take for granted that children actively engage
with their physical and social environment,
constructing cognitive models to make sense
of their changing environment and gradually
acquiring increasing sophistication in their
intellectual, social and moral understanding.
Studies of social development have emphasised
childrens role as social actors and meaning
makers (Bruner and Haste,
1987), partners in
social interaction, reciprocal exchanges and
transactional patterns of mutual influence
(reviewed by Schaffer,
1996). Meanwhile,
sociological theories have emphasised the
power of social structure to shape individual
lives, while micro-analysis of social process has
revealed the ways individuals contribute to the
creation of social life. Reconciling structure and
agency has been a major theme (Giddens,
1979)
that continues to underpin studies into childrens
socialisation, with a surge of interest in
exploring aspects of childrens social competence
(e.g., Hutchby and Moran-Ellis,
1998) as well as
in mapping the ways children construct their
socialisation (Mayall,
1994). Reconstructing the
young child’s status in childhood theory (James
et al.,
1998; Woodhead, 1998) has been matched
by reframing their role in research (Alderson and
Morrow,
2004; Woodhead and Faulkner, 2008).
Recent analytic interest in childrens agency
has considerable implications for child rights-
based research, policy and practice, including
around early transitions. Stressing the pivotal
role of childrens participatory rights, General
Comment
7 notes that: “Respect for the young
child’s agency – as a participant in family,
community and society – is frequently
overlooked, or rejected on the grounds of age
and immaturity” (
UNCRC et al., 2006: 40). This
also implies recognition of the fact that children
are active agents constantly involved in making
sense of and participating in the ongoing affairs
of their social surrounding (Woodhead,
2006:
28
). This chapter first examines research into
the role of peer cultures in childrens transition
experiences, followed by a summary of the
Mosaic Approach, a methodological tool for
studying childrens participation in their own
transitions.
Children’s peer cultures
Within contemporary developmental psychology,
socio-cultural theorists recognise that childrens
learning is guided not only by adult members of
their community but also through collaborative
learning with their peers (e.g., Mercer and
Littleton,
2007: 38). In a similar vein, sociological
research suggests that children creatively
Chapter 4: Children’s participation in transitions
32
appropriate information from the adult world
and produce autonomous peer cultures (e.g.,
Corsaro,
1992: 168). Corsaro and Molinari (2005)
report an ethnographic study of childrens
transition from pre-school to elementary school.
Their research focuses on how children
collectively prepare for their transition from
pre-school to elementary school through
‘priming events, a way of constructing initial
bridges between different settings through
events and routines (e.g., celebrations, singing
of special songs, etc.) which are initiated and
promoted by children, their peers, teachers,
families and community members. Peer activity
is given particular importance, as it is evidence
of peer influence in appropriating culture and
of children engaging in laying the foundations
for their future.
Through their concept of ‘interpretive
reproduction, Corsaro and Molinari highlight
childrens collective agency in shaping their
evolving membership in their culture, as well
as the ways in which their agency is socially
constrained. The ‘interpretive reproduction
approach draws a parallel with socio-cultural
theory when they acknowledge that changes
(including life transitions) occur when
individuals are involved in ongoing activities
with their environment. Similar to Rogoffs
idea of participatory appropriation, Corsaro
and Molinari’s ‘priming events are interactive
and symbolic activities that enable children to
actively contribute to experiences of change.
7
However, their approach departs from socio-
cultural theory in the explicit focus on the
constraints that shape childrens engagement
with the world and by foregrounding
ethnography as the key method for understanding
the interaction between peer groups, childrens
caregivers, and their teachers. Corsaro and
Molinari’s approach also stands in contrast to
ecological system theory, in that the singular
child’ is not placed at the centre of the model.
Instead, the collective character of transition
experiences is stressed as shared among peers at
every stage: in their anticipation of transitions
(e.g., through priming events), in the process of
transition and in reflections on past transitions
(Corsaro and Molinari,
2005: 20–22).
From a different study of school transitions, two
brief examples of children talking about their
experiences illustrate the point (Brooker, personal
communication). The first is from a child in
Bangladesh and the second is from Fiji:
Anticipation: “I heard that in the school where
I’m going, the older kids hit the younger kids. If
that happens I’ll come back to this school. And
if I have to go to another school I’ll go to a good
one. I wont go back to that one.
Reflection: “I did not really enjoy going into pre-
school. This is because my parents had done a
space in my home like a pre-school. I had a see-
saw, a swing, a pile of sand, and a lot of toys that
I can play with. My mother and father would tell
7
See also section on ‘rites of passage’
33
Childrens participation in transitions
stories and read story books to me before going
to bed.
Ethnographic research with young children
corroborates the importance of peer cultures
(Brooker,
2006; Pratt and George, 2005). For
example, comparative research in a Korean
private kindergarten and a
UK reception class
found that peer cultures influence considerably
individual childrens beliefs about the world.
Peer cultures may transform opinions on gender
roles and relationships acquired within the
family. In this way, peers may have a dual role;
on the one hand, they are a source of
empowerment, and on the other, they are a
source of risk – for example, through
discriminatory behaviour that excludes certain
children from the peer group. The study
recommends proactive intervention in cases of
discrimination, based on careful listening to the
discourses of young children and their peers
(Brooker,
2006: 125–126).
Another study explored how peer cultures and the
gendered attitudes towards friendship of primary
school students shaped their experiences around
transfer to secondary school (Pratt and George,
2005). This study found that all children, but
particularly boys, experience stress associated with
school transfer, peer acceptance and teacher
expectations. In the face of a new learning
environment, the concern to belong and conform
to a peer group was shown to be very intense and
to exceed other concerns (e.g., for academic success).
The study suggests that peers can be both a
distraction as well as a source of support in shaping
educational pathways (Pratt and George,
2005: 24).
Methodological tools for transitions
research
In Listening to Young Children: The Mosaic
Approach, Clark and Moss (
2001: 41) insist that
“listening must not wait until children are able
to join in adult conversations. Premised on the
assumption that children communicate in dif-
ferent ways, the Mosaic Approach was develo-
ped as a way to ‘listen to young children and to
involve their views and experiences in reviewing
early years services. The main study was carried
out with
3–4-year-olds over an 18-month period
in a
UK early childhood institution (incorporating
a nursery and a homeless families’ centre located
on the same community campus), involving
children, staff, and caregivers.
The Mosaic Approach is described by the authors
as (Clark and Moss,
2001: 5):
Multi-method – recognises the different
‘voices’ and skills of children;
Participatory – considers children to be
competent and experts on their own lives;
respects childrens views and also their silences;
Reflexive – includes children and adults in a
joint effort of interpretation; views listening
as a process;
Adaptive – can be applied in a variety of
early childhood settings; methods will
depend on the characteristics of the group,
such as gender, cultural backgrounds, skills
of staff or researchers, etc.;
Focused on childrens lived experiences –
moves away from a view of children as
consumers of services;
.
.
.
.
.
34
Embedded into practice – can be used for
evaluation purposes (‘listening as consul-
ting’) and can also become part of daily
practice (‘ongoing conversation’) in early
years institutions.
There are two stages to the Mosaic Approach:
The first stage involves a process of documentation
by children and adults through a variety of
techniques, including participant observation
and participatory research methods such as
child-led tours, mapping, and role play. Using
visual and other non-verbal methods may be
particularly effective in working with children
with limited language skills (including older
refugee children, for example).
Stage 2 consists of piecing together information
for dialogue, reflection and interpretation, with
each perspective or unit of data providing one
piece of the ‘mosaic. When practitioners and
parents listen to childrens perspectives, “it is
in the interpretation of the material gathered
that the possibility for greater understanding of
young childrens lives will emerge” (Clark and
Moss,
2001: 55).
Clearly influenced by the Mosaic Approach,
Dockett and Perry’s
(2005) Starting School
Research Project emphasises multiple
perspectives in researching childrens transition
to school. Indeed, socio-cultural research on
young childrens transition experiences points to
the importance of involving parents in transitions
processes. This strategy is particularly conducive
to encourage parental involvement in childrens
transition to school, especially where home
cultures differ markedly from those of school.
Involving parents in research is also important
in identifying the areas where children and
adults may differ (or agree) on expectations
regarding school transition, experience, and
achievement.
Dockett and Perry produced data consisting of
photographs with accompanying text,
transcripts of conversations, drawings of school,
videotapes of interactions; together these
conveyed childrens perspectives, experiences
and expectations about school (Dockett and
Perry,
2005: 517). They found that what matters
to children in transitioning to school is often
different from what matters to adults.
Furthermore, they found that “there will be no
one ‘best’ approach that suits all children or all
contexts” (p.
519).
In transitions research, recognising this complexity
involves considering the decisions that are made
for children by adults and listening to what
children have to say about starting school.
Summary
Research evidence for the pivotal role of
childrens peer cultures is programme-relevant.
In many parts of the world, classroom sizes are
large and children of different ages are often
instructed together. Given the scarcity of
teachers in these schools, many children in these
classrooms are often left unattended. This may
.
35
Childrens participation in transitions
lead to boredom, increased physical risk and a
decrease in learning motivation among pupils.
In such conditions, peers, who have the potential
to teach each other through participatory
instruction, are an underused resource.
Participatory research and programmes with
children during the early years are also relevant
to democracy. Through the adaptation of
democratic principles, nursery schools can
prepare children from the earliest years on to
become critically minded and tolerant citizens:
“Honouring young childrens rights to express
their views creates more effective policy and it
fosters stronger, more cohesive and inclusive
communities. In these ways it contributes to a
healthy democracy which recognises that
childrens rights are the human rights of any
citizen. (MacNaughton et al.,
2007: 9)
Example: Tai Wisdom Association (TWA)
Research suggests that institutions of education, including early childhood education, have the
possibility to be places of change. According to Peter Moss
(2007), early childhood institution
and programmes can indeed nurture participatory democratic practice. This implies an ecological
consideration of democratic practices on many levels, such as families,
ECCE institutions as well as
the federal and local level of decision-making. During programme planning phases, involvement of
young children and their caretakers ensures that children’s best interests are at the starting point for
services and programmes. It is important to learn in what kind of environment they feel at ease to
start learning. For example, child libraries run by the
TWA are designed in a way that reminds children
and their caregivers of the architecture of homes.
TWA found that children enjoy buildings that are
surrounded by a corridor where they can read, chat or just lie down and sleep. Concerning the location,
TWA also tries to establish its libraries at the centre of community life, easily accessible on foot by
even young children. Finally, also the timing of libraries is adapted to children and their caretakers’
schedules. Libraries are therefore closed during office and school hours, but open until late in the
evening when children have free time
(TWA, 2007a: 24).
37
This review has offered an overview of key
theoretical approaches that may aid in the
understanding of early childhood transitions.
We have argued that children experience a range
of personal, social and cultural thresholds that
may or may not harmonise with their transition
to school. Depending on their socio-cultural
environment, role and status, children are faced
with different decisions and responsibilities at
various moments of their lives. Childrens
success or failure in passing through formal
educational transitions cannot be assessed
without regard to local education practices and
socio-cultural context. In order to harmonise
ECCE programmes with local education practices,
it is important to assess local child-rearing
practices, how these are underpinned by cultural
beliefs, and to obtain knowledge of culturally
recognised transitions, i.e. rites of passage.
The review stressed that evaluations of good’
outcomes of development are always defined
socially and differ according to a community’s
culture, which includes its economic surpluses,
its system of subsistence and tools of survival,
and its political, economic, and religious
systems. It is therefore important that researchers
and practitioners avoid imposing an ideal
endpoint of development that reflects their
own values as opposed to local understandings.
On a more positive note, culturally sensitive
interventions may have the potential to change
repertoires of practice by enhancing childrens
and parents opportunities to access services and
participate in meaningful activities.
Furthermore, this review revealed the tendency
for conceptualisations and research traditions to
be linked to different disciplinary perspectives,
with the fields of education and psychology
dominating the ways in which transitions
research has been framed, especially as it relates
to institutional transitions, notably schooling.
Anthropological and sociological perspectives
can enrich transitions research through the
concern with understanding a much broader
canvas, encompassing personal, social and
cultural transitions in wide-ranging contexts.
Multi-disciplinary collaboration in transitions
research and practice may foster holistic
approaches that contextualise childrens
experiences of change within the broader, inter-
related contexts of their families, institutions,
and communities. Especially with global
initiatives like Education for All, grounded
understandings of cultural practices and
livelihoods may inform programming in relation
to pre-school and primary education.
The review underscores the value of using a
variety of conceptual and methodological tools
to achieve a holistic understanding of childhood
transitions. Methodologies incorporating
ethnography and multiple methods were
highlighted as potentially useful in adapting to
the different ways in which children communicate
Conclusion
38
and to their diverse social worlds. Longitudinal
research in particular may be capable of linking
early transitions with later outcomes over the
life course.
The central message of the review is that
greater transparency is needed to make more
explicit the underlying assumptions regarding
childhood and child development that inform
policy, programming and research. There is
“the perennial temptation to inflate the
significance of a particular theory or evidence
where it serves advocacy, which is ostensibly on
behalf of young childrens rights and well-being,
but frequently is also linked to particular visions
for early childhood, specific stakeholders or sets
of political priorities” (Woodhead,
2006: 6).
Research and practice around early childhood
transitions centres largely on institutional
transitions, particularly in relation to formal
care and education. Political, economic, cultural,
and psycho-social factors interact in shaping
childrens ability to access quality basic services.
Transitions research has the potential to unravel
these factors – at micro, meso, and macro levels
– that explain why some children have
opportunities for development while others do
not, as well as the directions of development
and their impact on life course trajectories. Few
actions on behalf of children are apolitical or
free from theory, and this review hopes to
highlight the need to make more explicit the
underlying concepts and visions of childhood
and child development that drive current policy
and programming on early childhood transitions.
39
Border crossing: In contrast to rites of passage, border crossing theory focuses on transitions that
occur on an everyday basis. It presumes domains of life’ (e.g., home, workplace, school),
separated by ‘frontiers, or borders, which individuals must successfully cross on a daily basis in
order to perform their domain roles (Campbell Clark,
2000).
Cultural thresholds: Key concept for rites of passage indicating the sequential passage from one
state to another (i.e., from preliminal to liminal to postliminal).
Developmental niche: A child-within-family focused concept inspired by cross-cultural research.
The developmental niche comprises:
1) caregivers’ belief systems (ethnotheories) regarding child-
rearing,
2) the material conditions and in particular spatial arrangements of child-rearing, and 3)
the actual practices of child-rearing (Super and Harkness,
1986).
Domain: In border crossing theory, domain refers to a sphere of life separated from other domains
on the basis of distinct social roles, responsibilities, and location (e.g., kindergarten and parents
workplace) (Campbell Clark,
2000).
Ecological theory: Informed by systems theory, provides a framework for understanding the
multiple contexts inhabited by the young child (microsystems), the significance of border crossings
between microsystems, the linkages between contexts (mesosystems), and the wider influence of
exosystems and macrosystems (Bronfenbrenner,
1979).
Guided participation: Describes the process whereby caregivers, teachers and peers assist children
in their development. While inspired by Vygotskian theory, Rogoff extends the concept of zone of
proximal development to stress the inter-relatedness of adults’ and childrens roles, and applies the
concept to cover teaching processes outside formal educational settings (e.g., weaving, cooking).
Vygotsky’s interest was primarily in the role of literacy skills in learning, while Rogoff is more broadly
interested in culturally valued activities that may or may not include literacy skills. Guided participation
is universal but the forms of its expression vary according to cultural contexts, settings and social
actors (Rogoff,
1990).
Interpretive reproduction: Conceptually similar to Rogoffs ‘repertoires of practice, though with
greater focus on collective processes (peer cultures), and less focus on individual experience.
Interpretive reproduction captures two mutually constitutive processes:
1) children (and human
Glossary: Some major concepts relevant to transitions research
beings in general) interpret the social world for themselves (stressing their agency) and 2) by
interpreting it and acting within the social world they reproduce a social order (stressing structure)
(Corsaro,
1992).
Life course theory: Closely related to the ecological approach to human development. Human
development is understood as a multi-level phenomenon, comprising structured pathways through
social institutions and organisations to shape the social trajectories of individuals (Elder,
1994). More
than any other framework, this approach emphasises that human development cannot be detached
from social history and also captures the cultural-historical context of risk and protective factors that
shape childrens vulnerability/resilience.
Liminality: The second of three phases of rites of passage, also referred to as a state of being betwixt
and between to highlight the suspended status of individuals who leave one role but have not yet
fully incorporated the new role (van Gennep,
1960; Turner, 1995). Programme interventions targeted
at this phase may be particularly effective as children are on the cusp of assuming new roles and
responsibilities and possibly shifting their trajectories.
Mosaic approach: A methodology developed for participatory research and consultation with young
children (Clark and Moss,
2001). It promotes a combination of verbal and non-verbal techniques
with children and adults to elicit childrens views on their care settings and lives. Because of its
participatory nature, it supports the principles of outlined in the
UN Convention on the Rights of
the Child
1989. It is especially relevant to research and consultations with young children about their
transition experiences.
Peer cultures: The culture shared by children and their groups of friends or classmates. The role of
childrens interaction in their collective appropriation of culture is stressed, and peer culture may play
a crucial role in moderating the stresses associated with transitions (Corsaro and Molinari,
2005).
Priming events: Occasions such as celebration, activities, speeches and information-sharing
opportunities that anticipate imminent transitions in childrens lives and are intended to prepare
children for change. Both children and adults engage in these events (Corsaro and Molinari,
2005).
Pupil careers: The process by which children manage their role and identity as pupils within the
institutional structures and practices of school systems. It is relevant from pre-school through to
school, leaving a framework for studying variations in transition experiences and outcomes on bases
other than grades and other standard assessments of success (Woods,
1990; Lam and Pollard, 2006).
40
41
Repertoires of practice: Deep-seated dispositions for activity and behaviour in a given setting, based
on individuals’ prior experiences in similar settings, and structured by their opportunities to access
and participate in these settings. They are applicable to the understanding of levels of continuity or
discontinuity between settings during periods of transition (Rogoff,
2003).
Reversibility/irreversibility: Terms that refer to the impacts of specific (usually adverse) experiences
on later outcomes. They are of particular interest for the timing and targeting of programme
intervention. Reversible outcomes can be corrected’ through timely and appropriate interventions,
while irreversible outcomes have a greater influence on shaping life pathways, including key transitions.
Rites of passage: The sequential process (i.e. preliminal, liminal and postliminal phases) marking an
individual’s change of status, usually involving a public ceremony distinguished from everyday life
through specific symbols and rituals (van Gennep,
1960).
Scaffolding: A Vygotskian concept referring to the structured assistance children receive from their
peers and adults (e.g., parents and teachers) in reaching new skills and developmental goals.
Scaffolding is progressively withdrawn as childrens competence increases (Wood, Bruner and Ross,
1976). It describes processes underpinning the zone of proximal development. It is also linked to
the concept of guided participation, but the latter places greater emphasis on the ‘learner’s as well
as the teacher’s contribution toward engaging in tools and signs to transmit knowledge and practical
routines. Scaffolding is likely most important at times of transition, when children are faced with new
routines and challenges.
Vulnerability/resilience: A theoretical framework to account for variations in childrens ability to
cope with adversities. Outcomes for children are the product of a combination of risk versus
protective factors, which in each case includes both personal qualities as well as environmental factors.
Quality early childhood programmes can be an important protective factor in reducing childrens
vulnerability (Rutter et al.,
1998, Luthar, 2003).
Zone of proximal development: According to Vygotsky (1978), the transition between learning and
development occurs in the ‘zone of proximal development, which is the distance between the most
difficult task a child can perform without help and the most difficult task s/he can do with support. It
is therefore through the instruction (see scaolding and guided participation) from teachers, adults
and more skilled peers that children develop and learn to negotiate successful transitions.
Glossary
43
Ageways (2007) Strong grandmothers, healthy communities, pp. 8–9. Ageways: Practical Issues in Ageing and
Development
69.
Ahmed, S., Bwana, J., Guga, E., Kitunga, D., Mgulambwa, A., Mtambalike, P., Mtunguja, L., and Mwandayi, E.
(1999) Children in Need of Special Protection Measures: A Tanzanian Study. Dar es Salaam, Tanzania:
UNICEF.
Alderson and Morrow (2004) Ethics, Social Research and Consulting with Children and Young People. London:
Barnardos.
Alexander,
KL, Entwistle, DR and Horsey, CS. (1997) From first grade forward: Early foundations of high school
dropout. Sociology of Education
70(2): 87–107.
Arnold, C., Bartlett, K., Gowani, S., Merali, R.
(2007) Is everybody ready? Readiness, transition and continuity:
Reflections and moving forward. Working Papers in Early Childhood Development, No
41, The Hague,
The Netherlands: Bernard van Leer Foundation.
Bell, John F. and Daniels, S.
(1990) Are summer-born children disadvantaged? The birthdate effect in
education. Oxford Review of Education
16(1): 67–80.
Bohan-Baker, M. and Little, P. (2004) The transition to kindergarten: A review of current research and
promising practices to involve families. Cambridge,
MA: Harvard Family Research Project.
Bolin, I.
(2006) Growing up in a Culture of Respect: Child Rearing in Highland Peru. Austin, TX: University of
Texas Press.
Boyden, J.
(1997) Childhood and policy makers: A comparative perspective on the globalization of childhood,
pp.
190–229. In: James, A. and Prout, A. (Eds): Constructing and Reconstructing Childhood. Contemporary
Issues in the Sociological Study of Childhood. London and Washington,
DC: Falmer Press.
Boyden, J.
(2006) Children, war and world disorder in the 21
st
century: A review of the theories and the literature
on childrens contributions to armed violence.
QEH Working Paper Series, No 138, Oxford: Queen
Elizabeth House.
Boyden, J. and Ennew, J. (Eds) (
1997) Children in Focus – A Manual for Participatory Research with Children.
Stockholm: Rädda Barnen.
Boyden, J., Ling, B. and Myers, W.
(1998) What Works for Working Children. Florence: UNICEF and Stockholm:
Rädda Barnen.
Boyden, J. and Mann, G.
(2005) Childrens risk, resilience, and coping in extreme situations, pp. 3–25. In: Unger
M. (Ed) Handbook for Working with Children and Youth. Pathways to Resilience Across Cultures and
Contexts. Thousand Oaks, London and New Delhi: Sage.
Bredekamp, S. (Ed)
(1987). Developmentally appropriate practice in early childhood programs serving children birth
through age
8. Washington, DC: National Association for the Education of Young Children.
Bronfenbrenner, U.
(1979) The Ecology of Human Development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Bibliography
44
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1986) ‘Ecology of the family as a context of human development: Research perspectives.
Developmental Psychology
22(6): 723–742.
Brooker, L. (2006) From home to home corner: Observing childrens identity-maintenance in early childhood
settings. Children & Society
20: 116–127.
Brooker, L and Woodhead, M. (2008) Developing Positive Identities (Early Childhood in Focus 3). Milton
Keynes: The Open University.
Bruner J. and Haste, H. (Eds) (
1987). Making sense: The child’s construction of the world. New York: Routledge.
Campbell Clark, S.
(2000) Work/family border theory: A new theory of work/family balance. Human Relations
53(6): 747–770.
Carr, D. (2002) Moral Education and the perils of developmentalism. Journal of Moral Education 31(1): 5–19.
Chelimo, N. (2006) Transitions in pastoral communities in Uganda. Classes under trees. Early Childhood
Matters
107: 36–37.
Clark, A. and Moss, P. (2001) Listening to Young Children: The Mosaic Approach. London: National Childrens
Bureau
Clark, A.
(2005) Listening to and involving young children: A review of research and practice. Early Child
Development and Care.
175(6): 489–505.
Corsaro, W.A. (1992) Interpretive Reproduction in Childrens Peer Cultures. Social Psychology Quarterly
55(2): 160–177.
Corsaro, W.A. and Molinari, L. (2005) I Compagni. Understanding Children’s Transition from Preschool to
Elementary School. New York and London: Teachers College Press, Columbia University.
Cunningham, H.
(1991) The Children of the Poor. Representations of Childhood since the Seventeenth Century.
Oxford and Cambridge: Blackwell.
Cunningham, H. and Viazzo, P.
(1996) Some issues in the historical study of child labour, pp. 11–22. In:
Cunningham, H. and Viazzo, P. (Eds): Child Labour in Historical Perspective.
1800–1985. Case Studies
from Europe, Japan and Colombia. Florence:
UNICEF.
Dahlberg, G., Moss, P., and Pence. A.R. (1999). Beyond Quality in Early Childhood Education and Care: Postmodern
Perspectives. London: Falmer Press.
Danby, S. and Baker, C.
(1998). How to be masculine in the block area. Childhood, 5 (2): 151–175.
de Berry, J. and Boyden, J. (2000) Children in adversity. Forced Migration Review 9: 33–36.
Doek, J.E., Krappmann, L.F. and Lee, Y. (2006) Introduction to the General Comment, pp. 31–34. In: Implementing
Child Rights in Early Childhood. The Hague: Bernard Van Leer Foundation
/ UNICEF / UN Committee
on the Rights of the Child.
Dockett, S. and Perry, B.
(2005) Researching with children: Insights from the Starting School Research Project.
Early Childhood Development and Care
175(6): 507–521.
Eggers, E.K. (1997) Historical Dictionary of Burundi. London: Scarecrow Press.
Elder, G.H.
(1974) Children of the Great Depression. Social Change in Life Experience. Chicago and London:
University of Chicago Press.
45
Bibliography
Elder, G.H. (1994) Time, human agency, and social change: Perspectives on the life course. Social Psychology
Quarterly
57(1): 4–15.
Engle, P. and Breaux, C. (1998) Fathers’ involvement with children: Perspectives from developing countries.
Social Policy Report: Society for Research in Child Development,
7(1): 1–21.
Erikson, E.H. (1950) Childhood and Society. New York: Norton.
Estep Jr, J.R.
(2002) Spiritual formation as social: Toward a Vygotskyan developmental perspective. Religious
Education
97(2): 141–164.
Fabian, H. and Dunlop, A.-W. (Eds) (2002) Transitions in the Early Years. Debating Continuity and Progression for
Children in Early Education London and New York: RoutledgeFalmer.
Fabian, H. and Dunlop, A.-W.
(2007) Outcomes of good practice in transition processes for children entering
primary school’. Working Papers in Early Childhood Development, No
42, The Hague: Bernard van Leer
Foundation.
Feldman, D.H. and Fowler, R.C.
(1997) The nature(s) of developmental change: Piaget, Vygotsky, and the
transition process. New Ideas in Psychology
15(3): 195–210.
Flavell, J.H. (1981) On cognitive development. Child Development 53(1): 1–10.
Giddens, A. (1979) The Central Problems of Social Theory London, Macmillan.
Good, C. V.
(Ed) (1973) Dictionary of Education. 3
rd
Edn, New York: McGraw-Hill.
Grantham-McGregor, S., Cheung, Y.B., Cueto, S., Glewwe, P., Richter, L.M. and Strupp, B..
(2007) Developmental
potential in the first
5 years for children in developing countries. The Lancet 369: 60–70.
Hagan, J. et al. (1996) New kid in town: Social capital and the life course effects of family migration on children.
American Sociological Review
61(3): 368–385.
Hammarberg, T. (1997) A School for Children with Rights. Innocenti Lectures, Florence: UNICEF Innocenti
Research Centre.
Harkness, S., Hughes, M. & Muller, B.
(2005) Entering the developmental niche: Mixed methods in an intervention
program for inner-city children, pp.
329–358. In: Weisner, T.S. (Ed) Discovering Successful Pathways in
Childrens Development. Mixed Methods in the Study of Childhood and Family Life. Chicago: University
of Chicago Press.
Hart, J.
(2004) Beyond struggle and aid: Childrens identities in a Palestine refugee camp in Jordan, pp.167–187.
In: Boyden, J. and de Berry, J. (Eds) Children and Youth on the Front Line. Ethnography, Armed Conflict
and Displacement. Oxford: Berghahn.
Harvard Family Research Project
(2007) Available online at: www.gse.harvard.edu/hfrp/projects/fine/resources/
storybook/research.html (accessed
20/09/2007).
Hockey, J.
(2002) The importance of being intuitive: Arnold Van Genneps The Rites of Passage. Mortality 7(2):
210–217.
Hutchby, I. and Moran-Ellis, J. (1998) Children and Social Competence: Arenas of Action. London: Falmer Press.
James, A. and Prout, A.
(Eds) (1997) Constructing and Reconstructing Childhood. Contemporary Issues in the
Sociological Study of Childhood. London and Washington,
DC: Falmer Press.
46
James, A., Jenks, C. and Prout, A. (1998) Theorising Childhood Polity Press, Cambridge.
Jha, J. and Kelleher, F.
(2006) Boys Underachievement in Education: An Exploration in Selected Commonwealth
Countries. London: Commonwealth Secretariat and Commonwealth of Learning. Available online at:
www.col.org/colweb/site/pid/
4309 (accessed 14/01/2008).
Johansson, I.
(2007) Horizontal transitions: what can it mean for children in the early years? Pp. 33–44. In:
Dunlop, A.-W. and Fabian, H. (Eds) Informing Transitions in the Early Years. Research, Policy and
Practice. Maidenhead: The Open University Press.
Jolly, R.
(2007) Early childhood development: The global challenge. The Lancet 369: 8–9.
Kagan, S.L. and Neuman, M.J. (1998) Lessons from three decades of transition research. The Elementary School
Journal
98(4): 365–379.
Katz, C. (2004) Growing Up Global. Economic Restructuring and Childrens Everyday Lives. Minneapolis and
London: University of Minnesota Press.
Kohlberg, L.
(1981) Essays on Moral Development. Vol. I: The Philosophy of Moral Development. New York:
Harper and Row.
Lam, M.S. and Pollard, A.
(2006) A conceptual framework for understanding children as agents in the transition
from home to kindergarten. Early Years
26(2): 123–141.
Lansdown, G. (2005) The evolving capacities of children. Implications for the exercise of rights. Florence:
UNICEF Innocenti Research Centre.
Leach, F., Fiscian, V., Kadzamira, E., Lemani, E. and Machakanja, P.
(2003) An Investigative Study of the Abuse of
Girls in African Schools. Department for International Development: Educational Papers. Available
online at: www.dfid.gov.uk/pubs/files/investudyafricaedpaper
54.pdf (accessed 14/01/2008).
Lenzer, G.
(2002) Childrens studies and the human rights of children: Towards a unified approach, pp. 207–227.
In: Alaimo, K. and Klug, B. (Eds) Children as Equals: Exploring the Rights of the Child. Lanham MD:
University Press of America.
LeVine, R.A., S. Dixon, S. LeVine, A. Richman, P.H. Leiderman, C.H. Keefer and Brazelton, T.B.
(1994) Child
Care and Culture – Lessons from Africa. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Lourenco, O. and A. Machado
(1996) In defense of Piaget’s theory: A reply to 10 common criticisms. Psychological
Review
103(1): 143–164.
Lloyd, C.B. (Ed) (2005) Growing up Global: The Changing Transitions to Adulthood in Developing Countries.
Washington, DC: The National Academic Press.
Lloyd, C.B. and Blanc, A.K.
(1996) Childrens schooling in Sub-Saharan Africa: The role of fathers, mothers, and
others. Population and Development Review
22(2): 265–298.
Luthar, S. (Ed) (2003) Resilience and Vulnerability. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
MacNaughton, G.
(2000) Rethinking Gender in Early Childhood Education., London: Allen & Unwin.
MacNaughton, G., Hughes, P. and Smith, K.
(2007) Young childrens rights and public policy: Practices and
possibilities for citizenship in the early years. Children & Society
21(6): 458–469.
Makin, L. (2006) Literacy 8–12 months: What are babies learning? Early Years 26(3): 267–277.
47
Bibliography
Mann, G. (2004) Separated children. Care and support in context, pp. 3–22. In: Boyden, J. and de Berry, J. (Eds)
Children and Youth on the Front Line. Ethnography, Armed Conflict and Displacement. Oxford: Berghahn.
Marsiglio, W., Amato, P., Day, R.D., and Lamb, M.E.
(2000) Scholarship on Fatherhood in the 1990s and Beyond.
Journal of Marriage and Family
62(4): 1173-1191.
Mayall, B. (Ed) (1994) Childrens Childhoods Observed and Experienced. London, Falmer Press.
Mayer, E.
(2007) Tomasitos Mother Comes to School. La mamá de Tomasito visita la escuela. Cambridge, MA:
Harvard Family Research Project.
McNaughton, S.
(2001) Co-constructing expertise: The development of parents’ and teachers ideas about literacy
practices and the transition to school. Journal of Early Childhood Education
1(1): 40–58.
Mercer, N. and Littleton, K. (2007) Dialogue and the Development of Childrens Thinking. London: Routledge.
Mooney, C.G.
(2000) Theories of Childhood. An Introduction to Dewey, Montessori, Erikson, Piaget and Vygotsky.
St Paul, MN: Redleaf Press.
Morrow, V.
(2003) Moving out of childhood. In: Maybin, J. and Woodhead, M. (Eds). Childhoods in Context.
Chichester: Wiley: The Open University Press.
Moss, P. (2007) Bringing politics in the nursery. Early childhood education as a democratic practice. Working
Papers in Early Childhood Development, No
43, The Hague: Bernard van Leer Foundation.
Okon, W. and Wilgocka-Okon, B.
(1973) The School Readiness Project. Paris: UNESCO.
Petriwskyj, A., Thorpe, K. and Taylor, C. (2005) Trends in construction of transition to school in three Western
regions
1990–2004. International Journal of Early Years Education 13(1): 55–69.
Piaget, J. (1978) The Equilibration of Cognitive Structures. Oxford: Blackwell.
Pratt, S. and George, R.
(2005) Transferring friendships: Girls’ and boys’ friendships in the transition from
primary to secondary school. Children & Society
19: 16–26.
Punch, S. (2003) “Youd better talk to my son because he knows more than me”: Primary education and youth
transitions in rural Bolivia. Paper presented at an inter-disciplinary research symposium Uncertain
Transitions: Youth in a Comparative Perspective, held at the University of Edinburgh,
27 June 2003.
Qvortrup, J., Bardy, M., Sgritta, G., and Winterberger, H. (Eds) (1994) Childhood Matters: Social Theory, Practice
and Politics. Aldershot: Avebury Press.
Ridgely Bales, S.
(2005) When I Was a Child. Childrens Interpretations of First Communion. Chapel Hill, NC:
University of North Carolina Press.
Rimm-Kaufman S.E. and Pianta, R.C.
(2000) An ecological perspective on the transition to kindergarten: A
theoretical framework to guide empirical research. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology
21(5):
491–511.
Rogoff, B. (1990) Apprenticeship in Thinking: Cognitive Development in Social Context. Oxford: Oxford University
Press.
Rogoff, B.
(2003) The Cultural Nature of Child Development. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Rogoff, B., Mistry, J., Göncü, A., and Mosier, C..
(1998) Toddlers’ guided participation with their caregivers in
cultural activity, pp.
225–249. In: Woodhead, M., Faulkner, D. and Littleton, K. (Eds) Cultural Worlds of
Early Childhood. London and New York in association with The Open University: Routledge.
48
Rogoff, B., Moore, L, Najafi, B, Dexter, A, Correa-Chavez, M, and Solis, J. (2005) Childrens development of
cultural repertoires through participation in everyday routines and practices. To appear in Grusec, J.
and Hastings, P. (Eds) Handbook of Socialization. New York: Guilford.
Rousseau, J.-J.
(1979) Emile or On Education. Harmondsworth: Penguin.
Rutter, M. and the English and Romanian Adoptees Study Team.
(1998) Developmental catch-up, and deficit.
Following adoption after severe global early privation. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry
39(4):
465–476.
Ryder, N.B. (1965) The cohort as a concept in the study of social change. American Sociological Review 30(6):
843–861.
Save the Children US (2007) La transición exitosa al primer grado – Un factor clave para el desarollo infantil tem
prano – Resumen Ejecutivo. Managua: Save the Children
US.
Schaffer, H.R.
(1996). Social development. Oxford, England: Blackwell.
Scott-Little, C., Kagan, S., and Frelow, V.
(2006) Conceptualization of readiness and the content of early learning
standards: The intersection of policy and research? Early Childhood Research Quarterly
21(2): 153–173.
Smart, C. and B. Neale (1999) Family Fragments? Cambridge: Polity Press.
Spiro, M.E.
(1982) Buddhism and Society. A Great Tradition and its Burmese Vicissitudes. Berkeley: University
of California Press.
Stephens, S.
(Ed) (1995) Children and the Politics of Culture. Princeton, New Jersey: Princeton University Press.
Super, C.M. and Harkness, S.
(1986) The developmental niche: A conceptualization at the interface of child and
culture. International Journal of Behavioral Development
9: 545–569.
TWA
= Tai Wisdom Association (2007a) Library for Young Children. An ECD experience from Thailand. Khon
kaen: Tai Wisdom Association.
TWA = Tai Wisdom Association (2007b) Story Book Project for Rural Children. An Experience of Changes in Rural
Thailand. Khon kaen: Tai Wisdom Association.
Tudge, J.R.H., Odero D.A., Hogan D.M., and Etz, K.E.
(2003) Relations between the everyday activities of
preschoolers and their teachers’ perceptions of their competence in the first years of school. Early
Childhood Research Quarterly
18: 42–64.
Tudge J. and Hogan, D. (2005) An ecological approach to observations of children’s everyday lives, pp. 102–122.
In: Greene, S. and Hogan, D. (Eds) Researching Childrens Experience. Approaches and Methods.
London, Thousand Oaks and New Delhi: Sage.
Turner, B.S.
(1993) Outline of a Theory of Human Rights. Sociology 27(3): 489–512.
Turner, B.S. (2003) Biology, vulnerability and politics, pp. 271–282. In: Williams, S. J., Birke, L. and Bendelow, G.
(Eds) Debating Biology: Sociological Reflections on Health, Medicine, and Society. London and New
York: Routledge.
Turner, V.
([1969]1995) The Ritual Process. Structure and Anti-Structure. Chicago: Aldine Publishing Company.
UNCRC (United Nations Committee on the Rights of the Child) (2006) A Guide to General Comment 7:
‘Implementing Child Rights in Early Childhood’ The Hague: Bernard van Leer Foundation.
49
Bibliography
UNESCO (2006) Strong foundations. Early childhood care and education. Education for all Global Monitoring
Report
2007. Paris: UNESCO.
Uprichard, E.
(2008) Children as ‘Being and Becomings’: Children, Childhood and Temporality’ Children &
Society
22: 4.
Van Gennep, A. (1960) The Rites of Passage. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Vygotsky, L.S.
(1978) Mind in society. The development of higher psychological processes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard
University Press.
Walker, S.P. et al.
(2007) Child development: risk factors for adverse outcomes in developing countries.
The Lancet
369: 145–157.
Watson-Gegeo, K.A, and Gegeo, D.W. (1992). Schooling, Knowledge, and Power: Social Transformation in the
Solomon Islands. Anthropology and Education Quarterly
23 (1): 10–29.
Wolff, P.H. and Fesseha, G. (1999) The orphans of Eritrea: A five-year follow-up study. Journal of Child Psychology
and Psychiatry
40(8): 1231–1237.
Wood, D., Bruner, J.S., and Ross, G. (1976) The role of tutoring in problem solving., Journal of Child Psychology
and Psychiatry
17(2): 89–100.
Woodhead, M. (1996) In Search of the Rainbow: Pathways to Quality in Large Scale Programmes for Young
Disadvantaged Children. The Hague: Bernard van Leer Foundation.
Woodhead, M
(1998) ‘Quality’ in early childhood programmes – A contextually appropriate approach.
International Journal of Early Years Education,
6(1): 5–17.
Woodhead, M. (1999) Reconstructing developmental psychology – Some first steps. Children & Society 13: 3–19.
Woodhead, M. (1999b) Is there a Place for Work in Child Development? Implications of child development theory
and research for interpretation of the
UN Convention on the Rights of the Child, with particular reference
to Article
32, on children, work and exploitation. Stockholm: Rädda Barnen.
Woodhead, M. and Montgomery, H.
(2003) Understanding Childhood. An Interdisciplinary Approach. London:
Wiley/Open University.
Woodhead, M.
(2005) Early childhood development: A question of rights? International Journal of Early Childhood
37: 79–98.
Woodhead, M. (2006) Changing perspectives on early childhood: Theory, research and policy. Background paper
prepared for the Education for All Global Monitoring Report 2007. Paris:
UNESCO.
Woodhead, M. (in press) Child development and the development of childhood. In: Qvortrup, J. et al. (Eds)
Handbook of Childhood Studies. London: Palgrave.
Woodhead, M. and Moss, P.
(2007) Early Childhood and Primary Education. Transitions in the Lives of Young
Children (Early Childhood in Focus 2). Milton Keynes: The Open University.
Woodhead, M. and Faulkner, D.
(2008) Subjects, objects or participants? Dilemmas of psychological research
with children. In: James, A. and Christensen, P. (Eds) Research with Children: Perspectives and Practices.
London: Taylor & Francis (
2
nd
Edition)
Woods, P.
(1990) The Happiest Days? How pupils cope with schools. London: Routledge.
50
Woods, P. and Pollard, A. (1988) Sociology and Teaching. A New Challenge for the Sociology of Education. London:
Croom Helm.
Yeboah, D. A.
(2002) Enhancing transition from early childhood phase to primary education: Evidence from the
research literature. Early Years
22(1): 51–68.
Yaqub, S. (2002) “Poor children grow into poor adults”: Harmful mechanisms or over-deterministic theory?
Journal of International Development
14: 1081–1093.
Yoshikawa, H. and Hsueh, J.A. (2001) Child development and public policy: Towards a dynamic system perspective.
Child Development
72(6): 1887–1903.
Yvosi, R.D. and Keller, H. (2007) The architecture of cosleeping among wage-earning and subsistence farming
Cameroonian Nso families. Ethos
25(1): 65–84.
Zelizer, V.A. (1985) Pricing the priceless child. The changing social value of children. New York: Basic Books.
Bernard van Leer Foundation
P.O. Box 82334
2508 EH The Hague
The Netherlands
Tel: +31 (0)70 331 2200
Fax: +31 (0)70 350 2373
www.bernardvanleer.org
About the Bernard van Leer Foundation
The Bernard van Leer Foundation funds and shares
knowledge about work in early childhood
development. The foundation was established in
1949 and is based in the Netherlands. Our income
is derived from the bequest of Bernard van Leer, a
Dutch industrialist and philanthropist, who lived from
1883 to 1958.
Our mission is to improve opportunities for children
up to age
8 who are growing up in socially and
economically difficult circumstances. We see this both
as a valuable end in itself and as a long-term means
to promoting more cohesive, considerate and creative
societies with equality of opportunity and rights for all.
We work primarily by supporting programmes
implemented by partners in the field. These include
public, private and community-based organisations.
Our strategy of working through partnerships is
intended to build local capacity, promote innovation
and flexibility, and help to ensure that the work we
fund is culturally and contextually appropriate.
We currently support about
140 major projects.
We focus our grantmaking on
21 countries in
which we have built up experience over the years.
These include both developing and industrialised
countries and represent a geographical range that
encompasses Africa, Asia, Europe and the Americas.
We work in three issue areas:
Through “Strengthening the Care Environment”
we aim to build the capacity of vulnerable
parents, families and communities to care for
their children.
Through “Successful Transitions: The Continuum
from Home to School” we aim to help young
children make the transition from their home
environment to daycare, preschool and school.
Through “Social Inclusion and Respect for
Diversity” we aim to promote equal opportunities
and skills that will help children to live in diverse
societies.
Also central to our work is the ongoing effort to
document and analyse the projects we support,
with the twin aims of learning lessons for our future
grantmaking activities and generating knowledge we
can share. Through our evidence-based advocacy and
publications, we aim to inform and influence policy
and practice both in the countries where we operate
and beyond.
Information on the series
Working Papers in Early Childhood Development is a
‘work in progress’ series that presents relevant findings
and reflection on issues relating to early childhood
care and development. The series acts primarily as a
forum for the exchange of ideas, often arising out
of field work, evaluations and training experiences.
As ‘think pieces’ we hope these papers will evoke
responses and lead to further information sharing
from among the readership.
The findings, interpretations, conclusions and
opinions expressed in this series are those of the
authors and do not necessarily reflect the views or
policies of the Bernard van Leer Foundation.
.
.
.
9 789061 951032
ISBN 978-90-6195-103-2